Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2019 Nov;23(11):938-951.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.003. Epub 2019 Oct 6.

Mnemonic Similarity Task: A Tool for Assessing Hippocampal Integrity

Affiliations
Review

Mnemonic Similarity Task: A Tool for Assessing Hippocampal Integrity

Shauna M Stark et al. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019 Nov.

Abstract

The hippocampus is critical for learning and memory, relying in part on pattern separation processes supported by the dentate gyrus (DG) to prevent interference from overlapping memory representations. In 2007, we designed the Mnemonic Similarity Task (MST), a modified object recognition memory task, to be highly sensitive to hippocampal function by placing strong demands on pattern separation. The MST is now a widely used behavioral task, repeatedly shown to be sensitive to age-related memory decline, hippocampal connectivity, and hippocampal function, with specificity to the DG. Here, we review the utility of the MST, its relationship to hippocampal function, its utility in detecting hippocampal-based memory alterations across the lifespan, and impairments associated with clinical pathology from a variety of disorders.

Keywords: discrimination; hippocampus; mnemonic similarity; pattern separation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Mnemonic Similarity Task.
A) Examples of stimuli during the incidental encoding and subsequent Old/Similar/New recognition task. Colored boxes are to illustrate conditions, but not used during the actual task administration. B) Examples of images for each of the lure bins, ranging from most similar (lure bin 1) to least similar (lure bin 5).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Old, Similar, and New responses for Targets, Lures, and New Foil items.
In young (A; 20-39), older (B; 60-74) and MCI (C) participants, LDI declines with age and MCI (D), while REC remains stable in healthy aging (E). Older adults who are impaired relative to young adults and those with MCI show different trajectories in discriminating lures across similarity bins (F).

References

    1. Bettio LEB et al. (2017) The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 79, 66–86 - PubMed
    1. Gallagher M et al. (2006) Individual differences in neurocognitive aging of the medial temporal lobe. Age 28, 221–233 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Gomez-Isla T et al. (1996) Profound loss of layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 16, 4491–4500 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Price JL et al. (2001) Neuron number in the entorhinal cortex and CA1 in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol 58, 1395–1402 - PubMed
    1. Campbell S et al. (2004) Lower Hippocampal Volume in Patients Suffering From Depression: A Meta-Analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 598–607 - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms