Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Oct 14;14(10):e0223506.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223506. eCollection 2019.

The effect of food anticipation on cognitive function: An eye tracking study

Affiliations

The effect of food anticipation on cognitive function: An eye tracking study

Michelle S Segovia et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

By randomizing the order in which participants perform a cognitive test and a food choice task in a controlled experiment, we investigate whether cognitive capacity can be enhanced by the simple act of anticipating food intake. Our findings show that overweight and obese participants exhibit an anticipatory food reward effect, which helped enhance their mental resources and improve their performance in a cognitive test. However, we find no anticipation effect among normal weight participants. Furthermore, eye tracking data reveal that food temptation, in the form of visual attention and emotional arousal is higher for overweight and obese individuals when they are cognitively impaired.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Accuracy on Raven’s test by treatment and BMI category.
Accuracy represents the proportion of correct answers in the Raven’s (cognitive) test; *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, and * p < 0.10; mean comparisons are performed using Wilcoxon rank-sum tests.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Attention level (temptation) towards food products by treatment and BMI category.
Attention (temptation) is measured as the difference in time visit duration between the regular versus the lite snacks (δTVD); positive values means that subjects spend more time looking at the lite snacks, while negative values implies the opposite.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Average pupil size during food choice task by treatment and BMI category.
Average pupil size is used as indicative of emotional arousal exhibit by subjects while looking at the snacks in the food choice task; *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, and * p < 0.10; mean comparisons are performed using Wilcoxon Rank-Sum tests.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Shiv Baba, and Fedorikhin Alexander. "Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making." Journal of consumer Research 26, no. 3 (1999): 278–292.
    1. Shah Anuj K., Mullainathan Sendhil, and Shafir Eldar. "Some consequences of having too little." Science 338, no. 6107 (2012): 682–685. 10.1126/science.1222426 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mani Anandi, Mullainathan Sendhil, Shafir Eldar, and Zhao Jiaying. "Poverty impedes cognitive function." science 341, no. 6149 (2013): 976–980. 10.1126/science.1238041 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mullainathan Sendhil, and Shafir Eldar. Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. Macmillan, 2013.
    1. Deck Cary, and Jahedi Salar. "The effect of cognitive load on economic decision making: A survey and new experiments." European Economic Review 78 (2015): 97–119.