Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2019 Dec 2;2(12):e1918023.
doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.18023.

Effect of Teaching Bayesian Methods Using Learning by Concept vs Learning by Example on Medical Students' Ability to Estimate Probability of a Diagnosis: A Randomized Clinical Trial

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Effect of Teaching Bayesian Methods Using Learning by Concept vs Learning by Example on Medical Students' Ability to Estimate Probability of a Diagnosis: A Randomized Clinical Trial

John E Brush Jr et al. JAMA Netw Open. .

Abstract

Importance: Clinicians use probability estimates to make a diagnosis. Teaching students to make more accurate probability estimates could improve the diagnostic process and, ultimately, the quality of medical care.

Objective: To test whether novice clinicians can be taught to make more accurate bayesian revisions of diagnostic probabilities using teaching methods that apply either explicit conceptual instruction or repeated examples.

Design, setting, and participants: A randomized clinical trial of 2 methods for teaching bayesian updating and diagnostic reasoning was performed. A web-based platform was used for consent, randomization, intervention, and testing of the effect of the intervention. Participants included 61 medical students at McMaster University and Eastern Virginia Medical School recruited from May 1 to September 30, 2018.

Interventions: Students were randomized to (1) receive explicit conceptual instruction regarding diagnostic testing and bayesian revision (concept group), (2) exposure to repeated examples of cases with feedback regarding posttest probability (experience group), or (3) a control condition with no conceptual instruction or repeated examples.

Main outcomes and measures: Students in all 3 groups were tested on their ability to update the probability of a diagnosis based on either negative or positive test results. Their probability revisions were compared with posttest probability revisions that were calculated using the Bayes rule and known test sensitivity and specificity.

Results: Of the 61 participants, 22 were assigned to the concept group, 20 to the experience group, and 19 to the control group. Approximate age was 25 years. Two participants were first-year; 37, second-year; 12, third-year; and 10, fourth-year students. Mean (SE) probability estimates of students in the concept group were statistically significantly closer to calculated bayesian probability than the other 2 groups (concept, 0.4%; [0.7%]; experience, 3.5% [0.7%]; control, 4.3% [0.7%]; P < .001). Although statistically significant, the differences between groups were relatively modest, and students in all groups performed better than expected, based on prior reports in the literature.

Conclusions and relevance: The study showed a modest advantage for students who received theoretical instruction on bayesian concepts. All participants' probability estimates were, on average, close to the bayesian calculation. These findings have implications for how to teach diagnostic reasoning to novice clinicians.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT04130607.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Brush reported receiving royalties from Dementi Milestone Publishing outside the submitted work; in addition, Dr Brush had a copyright issued for the book The Science of the Art of Medicine: A Guide to Medical Reasoning, with royalties paid. Ms Taylor-Fishwick reported receiving grants from McMaster University during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Participant Flow Diagram
The process of randomization, study completion, and analysis for students allocated to the 3 conditions.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Participants’ Estimates of Pretest Probability, Posttest Probability, and the Calculated Probability Based on Bayes Rule
Cases with negative test results and positive test results by whether the cases were learned cases or new cases, and all cases.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Difference Between the Calculated Bayesian Change Score and the Subjective Change Score for Each Intervention Group by Positive or Negative Test Results

Comment in

  • The Pursuit of Diagnostic Excellence.
    Centor RM, Geha R, Manesh R. Centor RM, et al. JAMA Netw Open. 2019 Dec 2;2(12):e1918040. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.18040. JAMA Netw Open. 2019. PMID: 31860100 No abstract available.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Elstein AS, Shulman L, Sprafka SA. Medical Problem Solving: An Analysis of Clinical Reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978. doi:10.4159/harvard.9780674189089 - DOI
    1. Kassirer JP, Gorry GA. Clinical problem solving: a behavioral analysis. Ann Intern Med. 1978;89(2):-. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-89-2-245 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barrows HS, Norman GR, Neufeld VR, Feightner JW. The clinical reasoning of randomly selected physicians in general medical practice. Clin Invest Med. 1982;5(1):49-55. - PubMed
    1. Ledley RS, Lusted LB. Reasoning foundations of medical diagnosis; symbolic logic, probability, and value theory aid our understanding of how physicians reason. Science. 1959;130(3366):9-21. doi:10.1126/science.130.3366.9 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Sox HC., Jr Probability theory in the use of diagnostic tests: an introduction to critical study of the literature. Ann Intern Med. 1986;104(1):60-66. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-104-1-60 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

Associated data