Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Jan 6;15(1):e0221000.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221000. eCollection 2020.

Musical expertise generalizes to superior temporal scaling in a Morse code tapping task

Affiliations

Musical expertise generalizes to superior temporal scaling in a Morse code tapping task

Matthew A Slayton et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

A key feature of the brain's ability to tell time and generate complex temporal patterns is its capacity to produce similar temporal patterns at different speeds. For example, humans can tie a shoe, type, or play an instrument at different speeds or tempi-a phenomenon referred to as temporal scaling. While it is well established that training improves timing precision and accuracy, it is not known whether expertise improves temporal scaling, and if so, whether it generalizes across skill domains. We quantified temporal scaling and timing precision in musicians and non-musicians as they learned to tap a Morse code sequence. We found that non-musicians improved significantly over the course of days of training at the standard speed. In contrast, musicians exhibited a high level of temporal precision on the first day, which did not improve significantly with training. Although there was no significant difference in performance at the end of training at the standard speed, musicians were significantly better at temporal scaling-i.e., at reproducing the learned Morse code pattern at faster and slower speeds. Interestingly, both musicians and non-musicians exhibited a Weber-speed effect, where temporal precision at the same absolute time was higher when producing patterns at the faster speed. These results are the first to establish that the ability to generate the same motor patterns at different speeds improves with extensive training and generalizes to non-musical domains.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Temporal precision in non-musician controls but not musicians improves significantly over the course of four training days.
(A) Schematic of the protocol and stimuli. (B) Sample data across four days for one musician and one control subject. The slope of the linear fit of the standard deviation versus mean tap time was defined as the Weber Coefficient. (C, D) Boxplots were plotted for all nine subjects from each group for the four training days for both normalized root-mean-square-error (NRMSE) (C) and Weber Coefficient (D).
Fig 2
Fig 2. Performance at the 1x speed on test day.
(A, B) On test day there was no significant difference between the musician and control groups for either the Weber Coefficient (A) or the NRMSE (B) at the 1x speed.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Superior temporal scaling in musicians.
(A) Magnitude of temporal scaling as measured by Scaling Factor, the ratio of the duration of the 1x pattern in relation to the duration of the 0.5x or 2x patterns. (B) Quality of temporal scaling as measured by Scaling Index (the correlation between the mean tap times in the 2x or 0.5x and 1x patterns). Scaling Index was significantly better in the musician group at both speeds.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Analysis of variance at scaled times.
(A) Example of the Weber Coefficient for a musician and a control subject at the 0.5x and 2x speeds. (B) Analysis of the Weber coefficients revealed main effects of group (Musicians x Controls) and speed (0.5x versus 2x). (C) The coefficient of variation (the ratio of standard deviation and mean) across all taps was significantly lower for the musician group at both speeds.

References

    1. Dowling WJ, Bartlett JC, Halpern AR, Andrews MW. Melody recognition at fast and slow tempos: Effects of age, experience, and familiarity. Percept Psychophys. 2008. April 1;70(3):496–502. 10.3758/pp.70.3.496 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Shankar KH, Howard MW. A Scale-Invariant Internal Representation of Time. Neural Comput. 2012. January 29;24(1):134–93. 10.1162/NECO_a_00212 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Lerner Y, Honey CJ, Katkov M, Hasson U. Temporal scaling of neural responses to compressed and dilated natural speech. J Neurophysiol. 2014. June 15;111(12):2433–44. 10.1152/jn.00497.2013 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Mello GBM, Soares S, Paton JJ. A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum. Curr Biol. 2015. May 4;25(9):1113–22. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.036 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Namboodiri VMK, Huertas MA, Monk KJ, Shouval HZ, Hussain Shuler MG. Visually Cued Action Timing in the Primary Visual Cortex. Neuron. 2015. April 8;86(1):319–30. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.043 - DOI - PMC - PubMed