Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Aug 31;25(2):257-272.
doi: 10.1080/13218719.2017.1364677. eCollection 2018.

Research-Based Instructions Induce Sensitivity to Confession Evidence

Affiliations

Research-Based Instructions Induce Sensitivity to Confession Evidence

Angela M Jones et al. Psychiatr Psychol Law. .

Abstract

Evidence is mixed on whether or not laypersons have sufficient knowledge of false confession risk factors. Procedural safeguards such as judicial instructions may assist jurors who are unable to effectively evaluate confession evidence. Participants were randomly assigned to one of five conditions that varied in the quality of a confession and the presence of instructions on coercive interrogation techniques. The results indicate that instructions induce sensitivity by altering verdict decisions and perceptions of evidence strength and confession voluntariness in line with the quality of the interrogation. Furthermore, the presence of instructions in low-quality interrogations resulted in participants completely discounting the confession. These findings suggest that research-based instructions on coercive interrogation techniques may be an effective safeguard against the use of potentially unreliable confession evidence.

Keywords: confession; interrogation; judicial instructions; juror decision-making.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

    1. Appleby S. C., Hasel L. E., & Kassin S. M. (2013). Police-induced confessions: An empirical analysis of their content and impact. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 19, 111–128. doi:10.1080/1068316X.2011.613389 - DOI
    1. Blandón-Gitlin I., Sperry K., & Leo R. (2011). Jurors believe interrogation tactics are not likely to elicit false confessions: Will expert witness testimony inform the otherwise? Psychology, Crime, & Law, 17, 239–260. doi:10.1080/10683160903113699 - DOI
    1. Bornstein B. H. (1999). The ecological validity of jury simulations: Is the jury still out? Law and Human Behavior, 23, 75–91. doi:10.1023/A:1022326807441 - DOI
    1. Buhrmester M., Kwang T., & Gosling S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Casler K., Bickel L., & Hackett E. (2013). Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon's mTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2156–2160. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.009 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources