Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Feb 6:8:e8425.
doi: 10.7717/peerj.8425. eCollection 2020.

Effects of personality and rearing-history on the welfare of captive Asiatic lions (Panthera leo persica)

Affiliations

Effects of personality and rearing-history on the welfare of captive Asiatic lions (Panthera leo persica)

Sitendu Goswami et al. PeerJ. .

Abstract

Background: The long-term success of ex-situ conservation programmes depends on species-appropriate husbandry and enrichment practices complemented by an accurate welfare assessment protocol. Zoos and conservation breeding programmes should employ a bottom-up approach to account for intraspecific variations in measures of animal welfare. We studied 35 (14:21) captive Asiatic lions in Sakkarbaug Zoological Garden, Junagadh, India to understand the implications of individual variations on welfare measures. We categorized the subjects based on personality traits (bold or shy), rearing history (wild-rescued or captive-raised), sex, and social-grouping. We explored the association of these categorical variables on welfare indices such as behavioural diversity, latency to approach novel objects, enclosure usage and aberrant repetitive behaviours. Further, we assessed the inter-relationships between different behavioural measures of welfare.

Results: Our results show that intraspecific variations based on rearing-history and personality traits are significantly associated with the welfare states of captive Asiatic lions. Asiatic lions with bold personality traits (M = 0.50, SD = 0.12, N = 21) and those raised in captivity (M = 0.47, SD = 0.12, N = 16) used enclosure space more homogenously compared to shy (M = 0.71, SD = 0.15, N = 14) and wild-rescued (M = 0.67, SD = 0.15, N = 19) animals. Behaviour diversity was significantly higher in captive-raised (M = 1.26, SD = 0.3, N = 16) and bold (M = 1.23, SD = 0.26, N = 21) subjects compared to wild-rescued (M = 0.83, SD = 0.35, N = 19) and shy (M = 0.73, SD = 0.34, N = 14) individuals. Aberrant repetitive behaviours (stereotypy) were significantly lower in bold (M = 7.01, SD = 4, N = 21) and captive-raised (M = 7.74, SD = 5.3) individuals compared to wild-rescued (M = 13.12, SD = 6.25, N = 19) and shy (M = 16.13, SD = 5.4, N = 16) lions. Sex and social-grouping of subjects did not show significant associations with behavioural welfare indices. Interestingly, behaviour diversity was reliably predicted by the enclosure usage patterns and aberrant repetitive behaviours displayed by subjects.

Discussion: Our findings underline the importance of individual-centric, behaviour-based, and multi-dimensional welfare assessment approaches in ex-situ conservation programmes. The results suggest that behavioural welfare indices complemented with individual variations can explain inter-individual differences in behavioural welfare measure outcomes of Asiatic lions. These findings also provide zoo managers with a non-invasive tool to reliably assess and improve husbandry practices for Asiatic lions. Understanding the unique welfare requirement of individuals in captivity will be crucial for the survival of the species.

Keywords: Animal personality; Behaviour diversity; Captive animal welfare; Cognition; Ex-situ conservation; Latency; Stereotypy.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare there are no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Schematic representation of an enclosure in Sakkarbaug zoological garden with the layout of zones for behavioural observations of enclosure use by study subjects.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Comparison of behavioural welfare indices of Asiatic lions across personality (bold and shy), life-history (wild and captive), sex (male and female), and social grouping (pair-housed vs group-housed) categories.
The behavioural welfare indices used here are (A) Enclosure usage; (B) Behaviour diversity; (C) Aberrant repetitive behaviour; and (D) Latency to novel objects.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ainsworth MS, Bowlby J. An ethological approach to personality development. American Psychologist. 1991;46:333–341. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.333. - DOI
    1. Allport FH, Allport GW. Personality traits: their classification and measurement. The Journal of Abnormal Psychology and Social Psychology. 1921;16:640. doi: 10.1037/h0069790. - DOI
    1. Altmann J. Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49:227–267. doi: 10.1163/156853974X00534. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Amato KR, Van Belle S, Wilkinson B. A comparison of scan and focal sampling for the description of wild primate activity, diet and intragroup spatial relationships. Folia Primatologica. 2013;84:87–101. doi: 10.1159/000348305. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Banerjee K, Jhala YV, Chauhan KS, Dave CV. Living with lions: the economics of coexistence in the Gir forests, India. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:1–11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049457. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources