Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2021 Aug 13;76(7):1264-1271.
doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa031.

Mind-Wandering Across the Age Gap: Age-Related Differences in Mind-Wandering Are Partially Attributable to Age-Related Differences in Motivation

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Mind-Wandering Across the Age Gap: Age-Related Differences in Mind-Wandering Are Partially Attributable to Age-Related Differences in Motivation

Paul Seli et al. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. .

Abstract

Objectives: A common finding in the mind-wandering literature is that older adults (OAs) tend to mind-wander less frequently than young adults (YAs). Here, we sought to determine whether this age-related difference in mind-wandering is attributable to age-related differences in motivation.

Method: YAs and OAs completed an attention task during which they responded to thought probes that assessed rates of mind-wandering, and they provided self-reports of task-based motivation before and after completion of the attention task.

Results: Age-related differences in mind-wandering are partially explained by differences in motivation, and motivating YAs via incentive diminishes mind-wandering differences across these groups.

Discussion: We consider these results in the context of theories on age-related differences in mind wandering, with a specific focus on their relevance to the recently proposed motivational account of such age-related differences.

Keywords: Aging; Mind-wandering; Motivation; Older and young adults; Task-unrelated thought.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Simplified clock face with hand pointed at the 12 o’clock position. The diameter of the clock face was 192 pixels. The clock’s hand made a complete revolution every 20 s.

References

    1. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01 - DOI
    1. Foster, S. M., Cornwell, R. E., Kisley, M. A., & Davis, H. P. (2007). Cognitive changes across the lifespan. In Qualls S. H. & Smyer M. A. (Eds.). Changes in decision-making capacity in older adults: Assessment and intervention (pp. 25–60). Danvers, MA: Wiley.
    1. Frank, D. J., Nara, B., Zavagnin, M., Touron, D. R., & Kane, M. J. (2015). Validating older adults’ reports of less mind-wandering: An examination of eye movements and dispositional influences. Psychology and Aging, 30, 266–278. doi:10.1037/pag0000031 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Jackson, J. D., & Balota, D. A. (2012). Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: Converging evidence from the sustained attention to response task and reading for comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 27, 106–119. doi:10.1037/a0023933 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Jordão, M., Ferreira-Santos, F., Pinho, M. S., & St Jacques, P. L. (2019). Meta-analysis of aging effects in mind wandering: Methodological and sociodemographic factors. Psychology and Aging, 34, 531–544. doi:10.1037/pag0000356 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types