Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Mar 4;15(3):e0229515.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229515. eCollection 2020.

P3a amplitude is related to conclusion specificity during category-based induction

Affiliations

P3a amplitude is related to conclusion specificity during category-based induction

Hong Wang et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Category-based induction involves the generalization of a novel property (conclusion property) to a new category (conclusion category), based on the knowledge that a category exemplar (premise category) has the respective novel property. Previous studies have shown that conclusion specificity (i.e., specific [S] or generic categories [G]) influences category-based induction. However, the timing of brain activity underlying this effect is not well known, especially with controlling the similarities of premise and conclusion categories between S and G arguments. In this study, the event-related potential (ERP) responses to category-based induction between S and G arguments were compared under both congruent (+, premise and conclusion categories are related) and incongruent (-, premise and conclusion categories are unrelated) arguments; additionally, the similarities of premise and conclusion categories between S and G arguments were controlled. The results showed that replicating this effect, S+ arguments have increased "strong" response rates compared to G+ arguments, suggesting that category-based induction is contingent on factors beyond matched similarities. Moreover, S arguments have more liberal inductive decision thresholds than G arguments, which suggest that conclusion specificity affects the inductive decision reflected by inductive decision thresholds. Furthermore, G+ arguments elicit greater P3a amplitudes than S+ arguments, which suggest greater attention resources allocation to the review of decisions for G+ arguments than that for S+ arguments. Taken together, the conclusion specificity effect during semantic category-based induction can be revealed by "strong" response rates, inductive decision thresholds, and P3a component after controlling the premise-conclusion similarity, providing evidence that category-based induction rely on more than simple similarity judgment and conclusion specificity would affect category-based induction.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. The experimental procedure.
Fig 2
Fig 2. The “strong” response rates, reaction times, and inductive decision thresholds for each kind of arguments.
S+ indicated arguments with specific congruent conclusion categories; G+ indicated arguments with generic congruent conclusion categories; S− indicated arguments with specific incongruent conclusion categories; and G− indicated arguments with generic incongruent conclusion categories. Error bars represent mean ± S.E.M. ***p < 0.001.
Fig 3
Fig 3. P3a amplitudes to the effect of conclusion specificity in the congruent and incongruent conclusions.
A) The grand-averaged waveforms elicited by G+ and S+ arguments and the difference waveforms between G+ and S+ arguments (G+ minus S+) in the anterior and posterior regions; and the grand-averaged waveforms elicited by G− and S− arguments and the difference waveforms between G− and S− arguments (G− minus S−) in the anterior and posterior regions. B) The topographies of the difference waveforms at 250–450-ms time window (G+ minus S+); and the topographies of the difference waveforms at 250–450-ms time window (G− minus S−). C) P3a amplitudes for each kind of arguments. The anterior region indicates the mean amplitudes of F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4, FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, and FC4 electrodes; the posterior region indicates the mean amplitudes of CP3, CP1, CPz, CP2, CP4, P3, P1, Pz, P2, and P4 electrodes. S+ indicated arguments with specific congruent conclusion categories; G+ indicated arguments with generic congruent conclusion categories; S− indicated arguments with specific incongruent conclusion categories, and G− indicated arguments with generic incongruent conclusion categories.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Pearson correlation analysis between the difference of P3a amplitudes (G+ minus S+) and the difference of “strong” response rates (G+ minus S+).
S+ indicated arguments with specific congruent conclusion categories; G+ indicated arguments with generic congruent conclusion categories.

Similar articles

References

    1. Heit E, Hayes BK. Predicting reasoning from memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2011;140(1):76–101. 10.1037/a0021488 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Hawkins GE, Hayes BK, Heit E. A dynamic model of reasoning and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2016;145(2):155–180. 10.1037/xge0000113 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Kemp C, Jern A. A taxonomy of inductive problems. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2014;21(1):23–46. 10.3758/s13423-013-0467-3 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Feeney A. How many processes underlie category-based induction? Effects of conclusion specificity and cognitive ability. Memory & Cognition. 2007;35(7):1830–1839. 10.3758/BF03193513 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Joiner C, Loken B. The inclusion effect and category-based induction: Theory and application to brand categories. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 1998;7(2):101–129. 10.1207/s15327663jcp0702_01 - DOI

Publication types