Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Feb 25:11:277.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00277. eCollection 2020.

Lay Beliefs About Interaction Quality: An Expertise Perspective on Individual Differences in Interpersonal Emotion Ability

Affiliations

Lay Beliefs About Interaction Quality: An Expertise Perspective on Individual Differences in Interpersonal Emotion Ability

Marcus G Wild et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Social interactions have long been a source of lay beliefs about the ways in which psychological constructs operate. Some of the most enduring psychological constructs to become common lay beliefs originated from research focused on social-emotional processes. "Emotional intelligence" and "social intelligence" are now mainstream notions, stemming from their appealing nature and depiction in popular media. However, empirical attempts at quantifying the quality of social interactions have not been nearly as successful as measures of individual differences such as social skills, theory of mind, or social/emotional intelligence. The subjective, lay ratings of the quality of interactions by naïve observers are nonetheless consistent both within and between observers. The goal of this paper is to describe recent empirical work surrounding lay beliefs about social interaction quality and ways in which those beliefs can be quantified. We will then argue that these lay impressions formed about the quality of an interaction, perhaps via affect induction, are consistent with an expertise framework. Affect induction, beginning in infancy and occurring over time, creates instances in memory that accumulate and are ultimately measurable as social-emotional expertise (SEE). The ways in which our lay beliefs about social interaction quality fit the definition of expertise, or the automatic, holistic processing of relevant stimuli, will be discussed. We will then describe the promise of future work in this area, with a focus on a) continued delineation of the thoughts, behaviors, and timing of behaviors that lead to high-quality social interactions; and b) the viability of expertise as the conceptual model for individual differences in social-emotional ability.

Keywords: affect induction; emotion; expertise; lay beliefs; social interactions.

PubMed Disclaimer

References

    1. Alden L. E., Wallace S. T. (1995). Social phobia and social appraisal in successful and unsuccessful social interactions. Behav. Res. Ther. 33 497–505. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00088-2 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bachorowski J.-A., Owren M. J. (2001). Not all laughs are alike: voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psychol. Sci. 12 252–257. 10.1111/1467-9280.00346 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Beaupré M. G., Hess U. (2006). An ingroup advantage for confidence in emotion recognition judgments: the moderating effect of familiarity with the expressions of outgroup members. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32 16–26. 10.1177/0146167205277097 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Beidel D. C., Rao P. A., Scharfstein L., Wong N., Alfano C. A. (2010). Social skills and social phobia: an investigation of DSM-IV subtypes. Behav. Res. Ther. 48 992–1001. 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.005 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bernieri F. J., Gillis J. S. (2001). “Judging rapport: employing Brunswik’s lens model to study interpersonal sensitivity,” in Interpersonal Sensitivity: Theory and Measurement, eds Hall J. A., Bernieri F. J. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; ), 67–88.

LinkOut - more resources