Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 May;5(5):520-529.
doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.12.021. Epub 2020 Jan 15.

Emotion-Cognition Interactions in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Increased Early Attention Capture and Weakened Attentional Control in Emotional Contexts

Affiliations

Emotion-Cognition Interactions in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Increased Early Attention Capture and Weakened Attentional Control in Emotional Contexts

Sarah L Karalunas et al. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2020 May.

Abstract

Background: Emotion dysregulation is a key dimensional trait in psychopathology. It is of particular interest in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) because individual differences in emotion dysregulation predict impairment. Despite growing recognition of its importance, an understanding of emotional functioning in ADHD needs to be better integrated with the well-known nonemotional attentional impairments in the disorder. Here, we assess differences in early, reactive and later, regulatory attention to emotional stimuli, as well as how impairments in attentional control to nonemotional stimuli are affected under different emotional contexts.

Methods: In all, 130 adolescents (nADHD = 61) completed an emotional go/no-go task while 32-channel electroencephalography data were recorded. Reaction time and accuracy were analyzed using the linear ballistic accumulator model.

Results: The multimethod approach provided convergent evidence of increased early, reactive attention capture and overarousal (faster drift rates, increased P1) by positively valenced stimuli in ADHD, but no differences in later attention to emotional stimuli. Overarousal in positive-valence contexts appeared to exacerbate existing ADHD-related impairments in attentional control to nonemotional stimuli as well (reduced N2 amplitude). In contrast, positive-valence contexts facilitated attentional control to nonemotional stimuli for typically developing adolescents.

Conclusions: Results highlight the dynamic interaction of emotion with attentional control in ADHD. Distinguishing reactive and regulatory contributions to emotion dysregulation has been informative for clarifying mechanisms and spurring the development of novel interventions in other disorders. It can be informative in ADHD as well.

Keywords: ADHD; Cognitive control; EEG; Emotion dysregulation; Event-related potentials; Sequential sampling models.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Disclosures

Dr. Karalunas, Dr. Weigard, and Dr. Alperin each reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Experimental Task depicted for the Happy condition.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Panels A and B show violin plots depicting posterior difference distributions for the effects of emotion conditions (Happy - Neutral, Fear - Neutral) for the Control (light grey) and ADHD (dark grey) groups. These are presented as box plots of difference distribution samples surrounded by density plots of the same samples. Panel C illustrates the effects of the positive emotion condition on the LBA model parameters for each group.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Effects of emotion condition on ERPs. Panels A and B show the ERP waveforms for each diagnostic group. Panel C shows the scalp topography for the emotion-neutral differences in each group. Scalp topography of the main effects for each emotion condition are in Table S1.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Effects of emotion condition on the no-go N2 ERP, which is related to cognitive control. Panels A and B show the ERP waveforms for each diagnostic group. Panel C shows the scalp topography for the happy-neutral condition. Scalp topography of the main effects for each emotion condition are in Table S1.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Beauchaine TP (2015): Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 44:875–896. - PubMed
    1. Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E (2014): Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 171:276–293. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F, et al. (2004): Impact of Executive Function Deficits and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) on Academic Outcomes in Children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 72:757–766. - PubMed
    1. Diamantopoulou S, Rydell AM, Thorell LB, Bohlin G (2007): Impact of executive functioning and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on children’s peer relations and school performance. Developmental Neuropsychology. 32:521–542. - PubMed
    1. Leibenluft E, Cohen P, Gorrindo T, Brook JS, Pine DS (2006): Chronic Versus Episodic Irritability in Youth: ACommunity-Based, Longitudinal Study of Clinical and Diagnostic Associations. Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 16:456–466. - PubMed

Publication types