Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2020 Sep;11(5):e1528.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.1528. Epub 2020 Apr 3.

Is atypical rhythm a risk factor for developmental speech and language disorders?

Affiliations
Review

Is atypical rhythm a risk factor for developmental speech and language disorders?

Enikő Ladányi et al. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2020 Sep.

Abstract

Although a growing literature points to substantial variation in speech/language abilities related to individual differences in musical abilities, mainstream models of communication sciences and disorders have not yet incorporated these individual differences into childhood speech/language development. This article reviews three sources of evidence in a comprehensive body of research aligning with three main themes: (a) associations between musical rhythm and speech/language processing, (b) musical rhythm in children with developmental speech/language disorders and common comorbid attentional and motor disorders, and (c) individual differences in mechanisms underlying rhythm processing in infants and their relationship with later speech/language development. In light of converging evidence on associations between musical rhythm and speech/language processing, we propose the Atypical Rhythm Risk Hypothesis, which posits that individuals with atypical rhythm are at higher risk for developmental speech/language disorders. The hypothesis is framed within the larger epidemiological literature in which recent methodological advances allow for large-scale testing of shared underlying biology across clinically distinct disorders. A series of predictions for future work testing the Atypical Rhythm Risk Hypothesis are outlined. We suggest that if a significant body of evidence is found to support this hypothesis, we can envision new risk factor models that incorporate atypical rhythm to predict the risk of developing speech/language disorders. Given the high prevalence of speech/language disorders in the population and the negative long-term social and economic consequences of gaps in identifying children at-risk, these new lines of research could potentially positively impact access to early identification and treatment. This article is categorized under: Linguistics > Language in Mind and Brain Neuroscience > Development Linguistics > Language Acquisition.

Keywords: developmental language disorder; dyslexia; rhythm; risk factor; specific language impairment.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared no conflicts of interest for this article.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Shared underlying mechanisms for musical rhythm and speech/language processing. (a) Models of underlying mechanisms for musical rhythm and speech/language processing emphasize the role of fine‐grained auditory processing, oscillatory brain networks and sensorimotor coupling (Fiveash et al., submitted). (b) Another line of literature emphasizes the shared role of processing of hierarchical structures in both musical rhythm and syntactic processing (e.g., Fitch & Martins, ; bottom figure is adapted from Heard & Lee, 2020)
Figure 2
Figure 2
Rhythm production ability and early literacy skills. Convergent evidence across data acquired with various methods, in support of associations between rhythm skills and language in preschool‐aged children (Reprinted with permission from Woodruff Carr, White‐Schwoch, Tierney, Strait, and Kraus (2014)). Children that performed well on a musical beat synchronization task (here called synchronizers, in red, shown on the rose plots on left to have better drumming accuracy) encoded speech more efficiently (top right) and show significantly better phonological awareness than their non‐synchronizer peers (bottom right). Synchronizers also performed better on a sentence repetition task (it is important to note that sentence repetition/imitation tasks not only require auditory perception and short‐term memory but also reflect deeper access to the grammatical structure of language: see Klem et al., 2015)
Figure 3
Figure 3
Rhythm perception and production difficulties in children with stuttering. (a) Children who stutter show impaired rhythm perception performance compared to TD children on a task requiring discrimination of simple and complex musical rhythmic sequences (Wieland et al., 2015). (b) Children and adolescents who stutter are less accurate than TD peers on synchronization tests, both tapping to a metronome at certain rates and tapping to the beat in music (Falk et al., 2015). Both figures are reprinted with permission from the original papers
Figure 4
Figure 4
Pleiotropy scenarios for shared versus separate genetic architecture of rhythm and speech/language. The Atypical Rhythm Risk hypothesis predicts that associations between rhythm and speech/language are (a) in part driven by genetic pleiotropy, such that a common set of causal genes affects both phenotypes directly, or (b) mediated genetic pleiotropy, such that genes directly affect rhythm phenotypes, and those phenotypes in turn affect individual differences in acquisition of speech/language during development, or (c) genes directly affect speech/language phenotypes, and those phenotypes affect individual differences in rhythm development. These models should be tested against the null hypothesis of separate genetic architecture. Moreover, a key to understanding the dynamics between genes, brain and behavior will be to test mediating neural endophenotypes linked to (d) shared or (e) separate genetic architecture

Similar articles

Cited by

References

FURTHER READING

    1. Haegens, S. , & Zion Golumbic, E. (2018). Rhythmic facilitation of sensory processing: a critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 86, 150–165. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.002 - DOI - PubMed

References

    1. Ajdacic‐Gross, V. , Vetter, S. , Müller, M. , Kawohl, W. , Frey, F. , Lupi, G. , … Rössler, W. (2010). Risk factors for stuttering: A secondary analysis of a large data base. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 260(4), 279–286. 10.1007/s00406-009-0075-4 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Alcock, K. J. , Passingham, R. E. , Watkins, K. , & Vargha‐Khadem, F. (2000). Pitch and timing abilities in inherited speech and language impairment. Brain and Language, 75(1), 34–46. 10.1006/brln.2000.2323 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Alm, P. A. (2004). Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: A critical review of possible relations. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37(4), 325–369. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001 - DOI - PubMed
    1. American Psychiatric Association . (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: APA.
    1. Amrani, A. K. , & Golumbic, E. Z. (2019). The Preferred Period Hypothesis Revisited: Rhythmic Preferences and Motor Tapping Precision in ADHD Adults and Controls. BioRxiv, 2019, 887802. 10.1101/2019.12.24.887802 - DOI

MeSH terms