Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Apr;147(4):2432.
doi: 10.1121/10.0001129.

Toddlers' fast-mapping from noise-vocoded speech

Affiliations

Toddlers' fast-mapping from noise-vocoded speech

Rochelle S Newman et al. J Acoust Soc Am. 2020 Apr.

Abstract

The ability to recognize speech that is degraded spectrally is a critical skill for successfully using a cochlear implant (CI). Previous research has shown that toddlers with normal hearing can successfully recognize noise-vocoded words as long as the signal contains at least eight spectral channels [Newman and Chatterjee. (2013). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133(1), 483-494; Newman, Chatterjee, Morini, and Remez. (2015). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 138(3), EL311-EL317], although they have difficulty with signals that only contain four channels of information. Young children with CIs not only need to match a degraded speech signal to a stored representation (word recognition), but they also need to create new representations (word learning), a task that is likely to be more cognitively demanding. Normal-hearing toddlers aged 34 months were tested on their ability to initially learn (fast-map) new words in noise-vocoded stimuli. While children were successful at fast-mapping new words from 16-channel noise-vocoded stimuli, they failed to do so from 8-channel noise-vocoded speech. The level of degradation imposed by 8-channel vocoding appears sufficient to disrupt fast-mapping in young children. Recent results indicate that only CI patients with high spectral resolution can benefit from more than eight active electrodes. This suggests that for many children with CIs, reduced spectral resolution may limit their acquisition of novel words.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIG. 1.
FIG. 1.
Proportion looking to the correct object for children in 8-channel noise-vocoded speech (experiment 1) and 16-channel noise-vocoded speech (experiment 2); the line represents chance performance, and circles represent data from individual children.

References

    1. Bashford, J. A. , Riener, K. R. , and Warren, R. M. (1992). “ Increasing the intelligibility of speech through multiple phonemic restorations,” Percept. Psychophys. 51(3), 211–217.10.3758/BF03212247 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bashford, J. A. , and Warren, R. M. (1987). “ Multiple phonemic restorations follow the rules for auditory induction,” Percept. Psychophys. 42(2), 114–121.10.3758/BF03210499 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bashford, J. A. , Warren, R. M. , and Brown, C. A. (1996). “ Use of speech-modulated noise adds strong ‘bottom-up’ cues for phonemic restoration,” Percept. Psychophys. 58(3), 342–350.10.3758/BF03206810 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baskent, D. , and Shannon, R. V. (2003). “ Speech recognition under conditions of frequency-place compression and expansion,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113(4), 2064–2076.10.1121/1.1558357 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baskent, D. , and Shannon, R. V. (2006). “ Frequency transposition around dead regions simulated with a noiseband vocoder,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 119(2), 1156–1163.10.1121/1.2151825 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types