Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 May 21;45(4):321-330.
doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjaa018.

Odor Sensitivity Versus Odor Identification in Older US Adults: Associations With Cognition, Age, Gender, and Race

Affiliations

Odor Sensitivity Versus Odor Identification in Older US Adults: Associations With Cognition, Age, Gender, and Race

Lucy Xu et al. Chem Senses. .

Abstract

The ability to identify odors predicts morbidity, mortality, and quality of life. It varies by age, gender, and race and is used in the vast majority of survey and clinical literature. However, odor identification relies heavily on cognition. Other facets of olfaction, such as odor sensitivity, have a smaller cognitive component. Whether odor sensitivity also varies by these factors has not been definitively answered. We analyzed data from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project, a nationally representative study of older US adults (n = 2081). Odor identification was measured using 5 validated odors presented with Sniffin' Stick pens as was odor sensitivity in a 6-dilution n-butanol constant stimuli detection test. Multivariate ordinal logistic regression modeled relationships between olfaction and age, gender, race, cognition, education, socioeconomic status, social network characteristics, and physical and mental health. Odor sensitivity was worse in older adults (P < 0.01), without gender (P = 0.56) or race (P = 0.79) differences. Odor identification was also worse in older adults, particularly men (both P ≤ 0.01), without differences by race. Decreased cognitive function was associated with worse odor identification (P ≤ 0.01) but this relationship was weaker for odor sensitivity (P = 0.02) in analyses that adjusted for other covariates. Odor sensitivity was less strongly correlated with cognitive ability than odor identification, confirming that it may be a more specific measure of peripheral olfactory processing. Investigators interested in associations between olfaction and health should consider both odor sensitivity and identification when attempting to understand underlying neurosensory mechanisms.

Keywords: aging; cognition; epidemiology; odor identification; odor sensitivity; social network.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Olfactory function, as assessed by odor identification (A) and odor sensitivity (B), among older US adults versus age, stratified by gender and cognitive function. Normosmia is defined as 4 or 5 correct for odor identification and 5 or 6 correct for odor sensitivity. For plotting purposes only, cognition was categorized using a MoCA cutpoint of 22.

References

    1. Adams DR, Kern DW, Wroblewski KE, McClintock MK, Dale W, Pinto JM. 2018. Olfactory dysfunction predicts subsequent dementia in older U.S. adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 66(1):140–144. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Attems J, Walker L, Jellinger KA. 2015. Olfaction and aging: a mini-review. Gerontology. 61(6):485–490. - PubMed
    1. Bengtsson S, Berglund H, Gulyas B, Cohen E, Savic I. 2001. Brain activation during odor perception in males and females. Neuroreport. 12(9):2027–2033. - PubMed
    1. Boesveldt S, Yee JR, McClintock MK, Lundström JN. 2017. Olfactory function and the social lives of older adults: a matter of sex. Sci Rep. 7:45118. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brand G, Millot JL. 2001. Sex differences in human olfaction: between evidence and enigma. Q J Exp Psychol B. 54(3):259–270. - PubMed

Publication types