Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Sep/Oct;53(5):354-365.
doi: 10.1177/0022219420920377. Epub 2020 May 26.

The Prevalence of Dyslexia: A New Approach to Its Estimation

Affiliations

The Prevalence of Dyslexia: A New Approach to Its Estimation

Richard K Wagner et al. J Learn Disabil. 2020 Sep/Oct.

Abstract

How prevalent is dyslexia? A definitive answer to this question has been elusive because of the continuous distribution of reading performance and predictors of dyslexia and because of the heterogeneous nature of samples of poor readers. Samples of poor readers are a mixture of individuals whose reading is consistent with or expected based on their performance in other academic areas and in language, and individuals with dyslexia whose reading is not consistent with or expected based on their other performances. In the present article, we replicate and extend a new approach for determining the prevalence of dyslexia. Using model-based meta-analysis and simulation, three main results were found. First, the prevalence of dyslexia is better represented as a distribution that varies as a function of severity as opposed to any single-point estimate. Second, samples of poor readers will contain more expected poor readers than unexpected or dyslexic readers. Third, individuals with dyslexia can be found across the reading spectrum as opposed to only at the lower tail of reading performance. These results have implications for screening and identification, and for recruiting participants for scientific studies of dyslexia.

Keywords: Bayesian models; diagnosis; dyslexia; prevalence; reading disability.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Funnel plot of residuals after regressing effect sizes on age.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
The distribution of the difference between listening comprehension and reading comprehension.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Scatterplot of listening comprehension and reading comprehension. Points to the left of the vertical line represent scores of poor readers (i.e., the 20th %-ile in RC). Points above the diagonal line represent readers with listening comprehension better than reading comprehension (i.e., at or above 1.5 standard deviations above the mean in LC-RC discrepancy score).

References

    1. Aaron PG (1991). Can reading disabilities be diagnosed without using intelligence tests? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24(3), 178–186, 191. dx.DOI.10.1177/002221949102400306 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Badian NA (1999). Reading disability defined as a discrepancy between listening and reading comprehension: A longitudinal study of stability, gender differences, and prevalence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(2), 138–148. DOI.10.1177/002221949903200204 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barth AE, Stuebing KK, Anthony JL, Denton CA, Mathes PG, Fletcher JM, & Francis DJ (2008). Agreement among response to intervention criteria for identifying responder status. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 296–307. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2008.04.004 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Becker BJ, & Aloe AM (2019). Model-based meta-analysis and related approaches. In Cooper HM, Hedges LV, & Valentine JC (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (pp. 339–363). New York: Russell Sage.
    1. Beford-Fuell C, Geiger S, Moyse S, & Turner M. (1995). Use of listening comprehension in the identification and assessment of specific learning difficulties. Educational Psychology in Practice, 10(4), 207–214. https://DOI.10.1080/0266736950100402 - DOI

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources