Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Aug;20(4):730-745.
doi: 10.3758/s13415-020-00799-0.

Sympathetic involvement in time-constrained sequential foraging

Affiliations

Sympathetic involvement in time-constrained sequential foraging

Neil M Dundon et al. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2020 Aug.

Abstract

Appraising sequential offers relative to an unknown future opportunity and a time cost requires an optimization policy that draws on a learned estimate of an environment's richness. Converging evidence points to a learning asymmetry, whereby estimates of this richness update with a bias toward integrating positive information. We replicate this bias in a sequential foraging (prey selection) task and probe associated activation within the sympathetic branch of the autonomic system, using trial-by-trial measures of simultaneously recorded cardiac autonomic physiology. We reveal a unique adaptive role for the sympathetic branch in learning. It was specifically associated with adaptation to a deteriorating environment: it correlated with both the rate of negative information integration in belief estimates and downward changes in moment-to-moment environmental richness, and was predictive of optimal performance on the task. The findings are consistent with a framework whereby autonomic function supports the learning demands of prey selection.

Keywords: Decision-making; Learning; Sequential foraging; Sympathetic stress.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Prey selection paradigm. Subjects decide whether to capture or release serially approaching invaders during their 2-s approach to the cockpit (panel A). Releasing an invader progresses immediately to the next invader, while capturing the invader incurs a capture time cost, and fuel reward. Four invader identities (panel B) map onto a two-by-two reward-by-cost value space, and can be described categorically as high (green), mid (blue), or low (brown) profitability. Subjects foraged for 12 min in each of two environments (panel C) with different proportions of invader profitability. Panel D: Replication of Garret and Daw (2019) learning asymmetry. Order of foraging (boom – downturn, BD; downturn – boom, DB) predicts optimal behavior (higher rank 3 captures in downturn relative to boom state). Learning deterioration of an environment takes longer than learning state improvement. Error bars illustrate the standard error of the mean across subjects
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Dynamics of a template heart beat (k), as measured by electrocardiogram (ECG; green) and impedance cardiogram (ICG; blue). Pre-ejection period (PEP) indexes sympathetic-mediated myocardial contractility, computed as the time between early ventricular depolarization (point Q on the ECG) and the opening of the aortic valve (point B on the ICG). Note that in our analyses we used a more easily identified ECG landmark for early ventricular depolarization (point R) and reverse-signed each estimate (see Methods:Physiological recording and preprocessing). Heart rate (influenced by both sympathetic and parasympathetic activity) is computed as the reciprocal of the R-R intervals
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Relationship between autonomic states, value and capture. Panel A: A single model of trial-wise choice returns a significant main effect where increased contractility (shorter PEP) increases capture (left panel) regardless of value and a significant interaction where increased low value capture is associated with decreased heart-rate (HR). Here, value is described as in Eq. [1], i.e., reward relative to opportunity cost in current objective rate of reward, with two levels of the continuous value (0 = low; 2 = high) modelled for illustration. As a guide, gray arrows above the plot describe direction of increased (+) or decreased (-) drive within the respective physiological variable. Panel B: PEP tracks deterioration in environmental richness. Only trial-wise changes in PEP dPEPdt significantly predicted trial-wise changes in environmental richness (dt), negative coefficient indicates that decreases in reward rate, i.e., deterioration, increased contractility (shorter PEP). Panel C: PEP predicts optimal learning. Blockwise changes in percentage of rank 3 captures (downturn – boom) modeled as a function of blockwise changes in PEP (red) and HR (gray); separately for mean physiological changes in the early (0–360 s) and later (360–720 s) portion of blocks. Optimal performance (higher D-B rank 3 score) predicted by higher relative drive in early downturn state, relative to early boom, only for PEP. *p<0.05; ***p<0.001

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Amemori KI, Graybiel AM. Localized microstimulation of primate pregenual cingulate cortex induces negative decision-making. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15(5):776. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823.
    1. Beissner F, Meissner K, Bär KJ, Napadow V. The autonomic brain: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis for central processing of autonomic function. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(25):10503–10511. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Benarroch, E. E. (1993). The central autonomic network: functional organization, dysfunction, and perspective. In Mayo Clinic Proceedings (Vol. 68, No. 10, pp. 988-1001). Elsevier. - PubMed
    1. Bernacchia A, Seo H, Lee D, Wang XJ. A reservoir of time constants for memory traces in cortical neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14(3):366. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources