Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Jul;85(5):1848-1865.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01364-7. Epub 2020 May 31.

Anomalous visual experience is linked to perceptual uncertainty and visual imagery vividness

Affiliations

Anomalous visual experience is linked to perceptual uncertainty and visual imagery vividness

Johannes H Salge et al. Psychol Res. 2021 Jul.

Abstract

An imbalance between top-down and bottom-up processing on perception (specifically, over-reliance on top-down processing) can lead to anomalous perception, such as illusions. One factor that may be involved in anomalous perception is visual mental imagery, which is the experience of "seeing" with the mind's eye. There are vast individual differences in self-reported imagery vividness, and more vivid imagery is linked to a more sensory-like experience. We, therefore, hypothesized that susceptibility to anomalous perception is linked to individual imagery vividness. To investigate this, we adopted a paradigm that is known to elicit the perception of faces in pure visual noise (pareidolia). In four experiments, we explored how imagery vividness contributes to this experience under different response instructions and environments. We found strong evidence that people with more vivid imagery were more likely to see faces in the noise, although removing suggestive instructions weakened this relationship. Analyses from the first two experiments led us to explore confidence as another factor in pareidolia proneness. We, therefore, modulated environment noise and added a confidence rating in a novel design. We found strong evidence that pareidolia proneness is correlated with uncertainty about real percepts. Decreasing perceptual ambiguity abolished the relationship between pareidolia proneness and both imagery vividness and confidence. The results cannot be explained by incidental face-like patterns in the noise, individual variations in response bias, perceptual sensitivity, subjective perceptual thresholds, viewing distance, testing environments, motivation, gender, or prosopagnosia. This indicates a critical role of mental imagery vividness and perceptual uncertainty in anomalous perceptual experience.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
a The ten faces viewed during training trials in all experiments, and in the main task of Exp. 2. b An example of a face embedded in noise from Face Set 1, used during training trials in Exp. 1. c An example of a face embedded in noise from Face Set 2, used during training trials in Exp. 1. d The checkerboard image that was used on attention control trials in Exp. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
A trend line (gray) and linear regression model (95% CI; gray shading) were fitted to the data for a Exp. 1a (N = 42); and b Exp. 1b (N = 37). Imagery vividness (mean rating on the VVIQ) is displayed on the x-axis and pareidolia proneness (binomial probability) is shown on the y-axis. Probability values were log-transformed for visualization purposes, so values that appear closer to 0 (log-transformed) are probabilities that are closer to 1
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
a, b Yellow-to-red colors indicate z scores denoting positive correlations between subject responses and pixel grayscale values, and light blue-to-dark blue colors indicate z scores denoting negative correlations for Exp. 1a (a) and Exp. 1b (b). The range of z scores shown in the figure was set to match that of Rieth et al. To compare our results to those of Rieth and colleagues, we zoomed in on the central grid location where the authors of the previous study performed their analyses, and compared the (non-significant) clusters contained in our CIs to theirs (c). This panel is reproduced, on a Creative Commons license, from Fig. 3 in Rieth, C. A., Lee, K., Lui, J., Tian, J. & Huber, D. E. Faces in the Mist: Illusory Face and Letter Detection in i-Perception, dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0421, volume 2, pages 458–476, 2011, SAGE Publications Inc
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
a A trend line (gray) and linear regression model (95% CI; gray shading) were fitted to the data of Exp. 2a (N = 25). The scatterplot shows the correlation between pareidolia proneness (number of misperceptions; y-axis) and imagery vividness (mean rating on the VVIQ; x-axis). b Two trend lines and linear regression models (95% CIs) were fitted to the data of Exp. 2a (N = 25) for correlations between pareidolia proneness (y-axis) and confidence (4 = high certainty; x-axis). The correlation with detection confidence is shown in grayscale, and the correlation with misperception confidence is shown in shades of red
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
a A trend line (gray) and linear regression model (95% CI; gray shading) were fitted to the data of Exp. 2b (N = 23). The scatterplot shows the correlation between pareidolia proneness (number of misperceptions; y-axis) and imagery vividness (mean rating on the VVIQ; x-axis). b Two trend lines and linear regression models (95% CIs) were fitted to the data of Exp. 2b (N = 23) for correlations between pareidolia proneness (y-axis) and confidence (4 = high certainty; x-axis). The correlation with detection confidence is shown in grayscale, and the correlation with misperception confidence is shown in shades of red

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aleman A, Böcker KBE, Hijman R, de Haan EHF, Kahn RS. Cognitive basis of hallucinations in schizophrenia: Role of top-down information processing. Schizophrenia Research. 2003;64(2):175–185. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00060-4. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aleman A, Nieuwenstein MR, Böcker KBE. Mental imagery and perception in hallucination-prone individuals. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2000;188(12):830. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200012000-00007. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Allefeld C, Pütz P, Kastner K, Wackermann J. Flicker-light induced visual phenomena: Frequency dependence and specificity of whole percepts and percept features. Consciousness and cognition. 2011;20(4):1344–1362. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.026. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Andrade J, Kemps E, Werniers Y, May J, Szmalec A. Insensitivity of visual short-term memory to irrelevant visual information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 2002;55(3):753–774. doi: 10.1080/02724980143000541. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Avons SE, Sestieri C. Dynamic visual noise: No interference with visual short-term memory or the construction of visual images. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2005;17(3):405–424. doi: 10.1080/09541440440000104. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources