Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Sep;85(6):2313-2325.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01400-6. Epub 2020 Aug 3.

The effect of preferred background music on task-focus in sustained attention

Affiliations

The effect of preferred background music on task-focus in sustained attention

Luca Kiss et al. Psychol Res. 2021 Sep.

Abstract

Although many people listen to music while performing tasks that require sustained attention, the literature is inconclusive about its effects. The present study examined performance on a sustained-attention task and explored the effect of background music on the prevalence of different attentional states, founded on the non-linear relationship between arousal and performance. Forty students completed a variation of the Psychomotor Vigilance Task-that has long been used to measure sustained attention-in silence and with their self-selected or preferred music in the background. We collected subjective reports of attentional state (specifically mind-wandering, task-focus and external distraction states) as well as reaction time (RT) measures of performance. Results indicated that background music increased the proportion of task-focus states by decreasing mind-wandering states but did not affect external distraction states. Task-focus states were linked to shorter RTs than mind-wandering or external distraction states; however, background music did not reduce RT or variability of RT significantly compared to silence. These findings show for the first time that preferred background music can enhance task-focused attentional states on a low-demanding sustained-attention task and are compatible with arousal mediating the relationship between background music and task-performance.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Inverted-U relationship between arousal, attentional states (mind-wandering, task-focus, external distraction) and performance, as hypothesised by Unsworth and Robison (2016) and in similar form Yerkes and Dodson (1908)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Schematic representation of a single experimental trial with a thought-probe after the trial
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Thought-probe response proportions as a function of attentional-state category and music-present/absent (music/no music). Error bars represent ± 1 standard error of the mean (S.E.M.)
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Thought-probe response proportions as a function of time-on-task (block number). Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Mean RT as a function of attentional-state category. Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Mean RT as a function of time-on-task (block number) and music-present/absent (music/no music). Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
Standard deviation of RT as a function of time-on-task (block number) and music-present/absent (music/no music). Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M
Fig. 8
Fig. 8
Each participant was assigned to the genre that made up the greatest proportion of time in their playlist. The graph represents the number of participants for each genre who listened to that genre for the greatest proportion of time. The ‘other’ category included tracks that could not be otherwise categorised, such as Japanese anime music, music for meditation, and some indie and folk tracks that did not clearly fit into a different main category. The ‘instrumental’ category included tracks that were primarily produced using musical instruments, such as remixes of original tracks, piano ‘lounge’ music, and ‘ambient’ music

References

    1. Amezcua C, Guevara MA, Ramos-Loyo J. Effects of musical tempi on visual attention erps. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;115(2):193–206. doi: 10.1080/00207450590519094. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Anderson KJ. Impulsitivity, caffeine, and task difficulty: A within-subjects test of the Yerkes-Dodson law. Personality and Individual Differences. 1994;16(6):813–829. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90226-7. - DOI
    1. Angel LA, Polzella DJ, Elvers GC. Background music and cognitive performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2010;110(3):1059–1064. doi: 10.2466/pms.110.C.1059-1064. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34(5):581–591. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources