Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Aug 11;15(8):e0236840.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236840. eCollection 2020.

Can gender inequality be created without inter-group discrimination?

Affiliations

Can gender inequality be created without inter-group discrimination?

Sylvie Huet et al. PLoS One. .

Erratum in

Abstract

Understanding human societies requires knowing how they develop gender hierarchies, which are ubiquitous. We test whether a simple agent-based dynamic process could create gender inequality. Relying on evidence of gendered status concerns, self-construals, and cognitive habits, our model included a gender difference in how responsive male-like and female-like agents are to others' opinions about the level of esteem for someone. We simulate a population who interact in pairs of randomly selected agents to influence each other about their esteem judgments of self and others. Half the agents are more influenced by their relative status rank during the interaction than the others. Without prejudice, stereotypes, segregation, or categorization, our model produces inter-group inequality of self-esteem and status that is stable, consensual, and exhibits characteristics of glass ceiling effects. Outcomes are not affected by relative group size. We discuss implications for group orientation to dominance and individuals' motivations to exchange.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. On the left, perceived dissimilarity with someone thinking that it is important to listen to people however, for different sexes and in different countries.
On the right, perceived dissimilarity with someone thinking that it is important to treat people equally. Source: ESS8-2016 data, ed.1.0.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Examples of credibilities (i.e. strength of the influence) given by an agent i to an agent interlocutor j depending on the absolute perceived distance i between j and him-herself (on the x-axis), and the fact i hold j in lower esteem (plain lines) or higher esteem (dotted lines), for σ = 0.1 (in blue), 0.5 (in orange), 1 (in green) and 3 (in red).
We observe that the higher the perceived distance, the lower the credibility for j when held in lower esteem, or the higher credibility for j when held in higher esteem. Moreover, the larger σ, the smaller the difference of given credibility to j if he/she is held in low compared to high esteem.
Fig 3
Fig 3
Upper plot: average group reputation for different values of σS. Lower left plot: average self-esteem of the groups for different values of σS. Lower right plot: Intra-group variability—variation coefficient for different values of σS. The black baseline represents a single group with open–mindedness parameter σS. In the other cases the population is divided into two equal-size groups with open–mindedness parameter σS (blue lines) and σL = f(σS) (magenta lines). The empty symbols correspond to the function: σL = σS + 0.1 the others to the case σL = 1.5σS.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Group S (in magenta) is less open-minded than Group L (in blue), and then occupies on average higher ranks (1 is the best rank).
Plot A: Boxplot of status rank values for different populations having different values of the open–mindedness parameter σS for the group S, and the corresponding value of (σL = 1.5 x σS) for the group L. Dashed lines indicate group means for the lowest σS 0.05, across levels of σS: we observe the difference of average ranking for group S and group L increases with the value of σS. Plot B: Probability for a rank to be occupied by an agent in a group at each level of the open-mindedness parameters (see legend, still with σL = 1.5 x σS). Plot C: Probability of moving to a lower status position (higher in rank) given the agent’s initial rank, by group and open-mindedness parameter. The three plots represent averages computed from 100 replicas of each tested parameter set.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Z-scores, indicating how different from the expected probability from the fraction of L in the population, is the probability of the more open-minded Group L being in the 5 most prestigious ranks (green points, lower than the expected z-score 0) and of being in the 5 least prestigious ranks (orange points, higher from the expected z-score 0) plotted by their proportion of the population of agents.
Each small point represents a simulation setup replicated 50 times, varying the combined values of (σS, (σL, k)—see the method section for details.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Credibility given to an interlocutor for whom our esteem varies around our self-esteem by 0.1 for different values of the open-mindedness σ; for a variation of: -0.1 implying the interlocutor is held in lower esteem (red square), or +0.1 implying the interlocutor is held is higher esteem (blue triangle).

References

    1. Sidanius J, Pratto F. Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social oppression and hierarchy. England: Cambridge University Press; 1999.
    1. Buss DM, Schmitt DO. Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review. 1993;100:204–232. - PubMed
    1. Harris M. The evolution of gender hierarchies: A trial formulation In: Miller D, editor. Sex and gender hierarchies. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1993. p. 57–79.
    1. Sacks K. Engels revisited: Women, the organization of production, and private property In: Rosaldo MZ, Lamphere L, editors. Women, culture, and society. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press; 1974. p. 189–206.
    1. Friedl E. Women and men: An anthropologist’s view. NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston; 1975.