Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2020 Jul 29:11:1347.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01347. eCollection 2020.

Ecological∼Enactivism Through the Lens of Japanese Philosophy

Affiliations
Review

Ecological∼Enactivism Through the Lens of Japanese Philosophy

Jonathan McKinney. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

The enactive and ecological approaches to embodied cognitive science are on a collision course. While both draw inspiration from similar views in psychology and phenomenology, the two approaches initially held seemingly contradictory views and points of focus. Early enactivists saw value in the ecological approach but insisted that the two schools remain distinct. While ecological psychology challenged the common foes of mental representation and mind-body dualism, it seemingly did so at the cost of the autonomy of the agent. This is evidence that the early enactive and ecological approaches told different stories about how agents and environments interact. Whereas the enactive approach broadly focuses on agency and the organism's resilience to environmental perturbations, the ecological approach insists that organisms are best understood in terms of the organism-environment system and at the ecological scale. Historically, this tension created space for harsh criticisms from both sides and for some ecological psychologists to dismiss enactivism altogether. Despite their differences, both approaches use dynamic systems theory to explain the interactions between embodied agents and the environment or contextual milieu in which they are embedded. This has led some scholars to focus on the complementary elements of each approach and argue that the two schools are allies, thus rejecting the historical disagreements between the two approaches and calling for an ecological-enactive synthesis. The attempts to synthesize the approaches are noteworthy and should be considered steps in the right direction but are potentially problematic. If the two schools are merely synthesized to some form of ecological-enactivism, then something of value from both approaches could be lost. This is analogous to the hasty comparison between two seemingly similar schools of thought found in early attempts at East-West comparative philosophy. I argue that the relationship between the enactive and ecological approaches is both complementary and contrary and is thus best understood in terms of complementarity. Given the complexity of complementarity I will unpack the notion in steps. I will begin with the exploration of analogous concepts in Japanese Philosophy and gradually build a lens through which both agent environment and ecological enactive complementarities can be understood.

Keywords: Japanese philosophy; complementarity; ecological psycholgy; embodied cognition; enactivism.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
This is an example of the figure–ground relation portrayed by the psychologist Edgar Rubin (See Pind, 2014, pp. 214-218). Interestingly, Rubin was influenced by Niels Bohr, the physicist responsible for complementarity in quantum mechanics. In likeness of Rubin’s Vase and other figure–ground images, Niels Bohr chose the Chinese yin-yang symbol for his coat of arms upon being recognized for his achievements in Denmark. It read “Contraria sunt complementa (opposites are complementary).” For more information about the crossover of figure–ground in perception, complementarity in epistemology, and nonduality in non-Western philosophy, see Pind (2014, pp. 204-210). Image attribution – Nevit Dilmen/CC BY-SA (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0).
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Splashed Ink Landscape (formula image) Haboku sansui by Sesshū Tōyō (1420-1506). Image attribution: Sesshū (1495), Tokyo National Museum, Tokyo, Japan (Wikimedia Commons).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baggs E., Chemero A. (2018). Radical embodiment in two directions. Synthese. 10.1007/s11229-018-02020-9 - DOI
    1. Chatterjee A. (2011). “Funes and categorization in an abstraction-free world,” in Apoha: Buddhist Nominalism and Human Cognition, eds Siderits M., Chakrabarti A. (New York, NY: Columbia University press; ), 247–257.
    1. Chemero A. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    1. Chemero A. (2016). Sensorimotor empathy. J. Conscious. Stud. 23 138–152.
    1. Costantini M., Stapleton M. (2015). How the body narrows the interaction with the environment. Percept. Emot. Embod. Found. Embod. Cogn. 1:181.

LinkOut - more resources