Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Sep 3;3(1):18.
doi: 10.5334/joc.61.

Self-Monitoring in Speaking: In Defense of a Comprehension-Based Account

Affiliations

Self-Monitoring in Speaking: In Defense of a Comprehension-Based Account

Ardi Roelofs. J Cogn. .

Abstract

Speakers occasionally make speech errors, which may be detected and corrected. According to the comprehension-based account proposed by Levelt, Roelofs, and Meyer (1999) and Roelofs (2004), speakers detect errors by using their speech comprehension system for the monitoring of overt as well as inner speech. According to the production-based account of Nozari, Dell, and Schwartz (2011), speakers may use their comprehension system for external monitoring but error detection in internal monitoring is based on the amount of conflict within the speech production system, assessed by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Here, I address three main arguments of Nozari et al. and Nozari and Novick (2017) against a comprehension-based account of internal monitoring, which concern cross-talk interference between inner and overt speech, a double dissociation between comprehension and self-monitoring ability in patients with aphasia, and a domain-general error-related negativity in the ACC that is allegedly independent of conscious awareness. I argue that none of the arguments are conclusive, and conclude that comprehension-based monitoring remains a viable account of self-monitoring in speaking.

Keywords: Auditory word processing; Executive functions; Eye movements; Language production; Neuropsychology; Response accuracy.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The author has no competing interests to declare.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Illustration of comprehension-based self-monitoring in WEAVER++. (A) In naming a picture, the phonological word representation of the picture name is fed into the speech comprehension system (the internal loop), which also processes the overtly articulated picture name (the external loop). The monitor compares selected production and comprehension representations. (B) Network representing the word cat, whereby output phonemes activate input phonemes, which serves the internal monitoring loop.

References

    1. Aarts, E., Roelofs, A., & Van Turennout, M. (2008). Anticipatory activity in anterior cingulate cortex can be independent of conflict and error likelihood. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 4671–4678. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4400-07.2008 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Blackmer, E. R., & Mitton, J. L. (1991). Theories of monitoring and the timing of repairs in spontaneous speech. Cognition, 39, 173–194. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90052-6 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Burle, B., Roger, C., Allain, S., Vidal, F., & Hasbroucq, T. (2008). Error negativity does not reflect conflict: A reappraisal of conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1637–1655. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20110 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Coles, M. G., Scheffers, M. K., & Holroyd, C. B. (2001). Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing. Biological Psychology, 56, 173–189. DOI: 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00076-X - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources