Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Sep 10;3(1):28.
doi: 10.5334/joc.117.

Erasing the Homunculus as an Ongoing Mission: A Reply to the Commentaries

Affiliations

Erasing the Homunculus as an Ongoing Mission: A Reply to the Commentaries

James R Schmidt et al. J Cogn. .

Abstract

In our recent article (Schmidt, Liefooghe, & De Houwer, 2020, this volume), we presented an adaptation of the Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model for simulating instruction following and task-switching behaviour. In this paper, we respond to five commentaries on our article: Monsell & McLaren (2020), Koch & Lavric (2020), Meiran (2020), Longman (2020), and Pfeuffer (2020). The commentaries discuss potential future modelling goals, deeper reflections on cognitive control, and some potential challenges for our theoretical perspective and associated model. We focus primarily on the latter. In particular, we clarify that we (a) acknowledge the role of cognitive control in task switching, and (b) are arguing that certain task-switching effects do not serve as a good measure of said cognitive control. We also discuss some ambiguities in terminological uses (e.g., the meaning of "task-set reconfiguration"), along with some future experimental and modelling research directions.

Keywords: binding; computational modelling; episodic memory; feature integration; goals; instruction implementation; neural networks; switch costs; task-rule congruency.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The author has no competing interests to declare.

Similar articles

References

    1. Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. L. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks In Umiltà C. & Moscovitch M. (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV (15th ed., pp. 421–452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    1. Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). Episodic and semantic components of the compound-stimulus strategy in the explicit task-cuing procedure. Memory & Cognition, 32, 965–978. DOI: 10.3758/BF03196874 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J. C., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 769–783. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Dreisbach, G., Goschke, T., & Haider, H. (2006). Implicit task sets in task switching? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1221–1233. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1221 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Dreisbach, G., Goschke, T., & Haider, H. (2007). The role of task rules and stimulus-response mappings in the task switching paradigm. Psychological Research, 71, 383–392. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-005-0041-3 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources