Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Apr;49(3):600-612.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-020-01103-8. Epub 2020 Oct 6.

Orthographic properties of distractors do influence phonological Stroop effects: Evidence from Japanese Romaji distractors

Affiliations

Orthographic properties of distractors do influence phonological Stroop effects: Evidence from Japanese Romaji distractors

Masahiro Yoshihara et al. Mem Cognit. 2021 Apr.

Abstract

In attempting to understand mental processes, it is important to use a task that appropriately reflects the underlying processes being investigated. Recently, Verdonschot and Kinoshita (Memory & Cognition, 46, 410-425, 2018) proposed that a variant of the Stroop task-the "phonological Stroop task"-might be a suitable tool for investigating speech production. The major advantage of this task is that the task is apparently not affected by the orthographic properties of the stimuli, unlike other, commonly used, tasks (e.g., associative-cuing and word-reading tasks). The viability of this proposal was examined in the present experiments by manipulating the script types of Japanese distractors. For Romaji distractors (e.g., "kushi"), color-naming responses were faster when the initial phoneme was shared between the color name and the distractor than when the initial phonemes were different, thereby showing a phoneme-based phonological Stroop effect (Experiment 1). In contrast, no such effect was observed when the same distractors were presented in Katakana (e.g., "くし"), replicating Verdonschot and Kinoshita's original results (Experiment 2). A phoneme-based effect was again found when the Katakana distractors used in Verdonschot and Kinoshita's original study were transcribed and presented in Romaji (Experiment 3). Because the observation of a phonemic effect directly depended on the orthographic properties of the distractor stimuli, we conclude that the phonological Stroop task is also susceptible to orthographic influences.

Keywords: Japanese language; Phoneme; Phonological Stroop tasks; Romaji.

PubMed Disclaimer

References

    1. Alario, F.-X., Perre, L., Castel, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2007). The role of orthography in speech production revisited. Cognition, 102, 464–475. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.02.002 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Amano, N. & Kondo, K. (2003). NTT Detabesu Shirizu: Nihongo No Goi-tokusei Dai 2-ki CD-ROM-ban [NTT database series: Lexical Properties of Japanese, Vol.2, CD-ROM Version.] Tokyo, Japan: Sanseido.
    1. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 - DOI
    1. Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, R. H. (2015a). Parsimonious mixed models. Available from arXiv:1506.04967 (stat.ME).
    1. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015b). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. doi: https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 - DOI

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources