Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Dec;69(4):802-838.
doi: 10.1111/lang.12352. Epub 2019 May 16.

Phonological Awareness Trajectories: Young Spanish-English and Cantonese-English Bilinguals

Affiliations

Phonological Awareness Trajectories: Young Spanish-English and Cantonese-English Bilinguals

Yuuko Uchikoshi. Lang Learn. 2019 Dec.

Abstract

This study focused on the phonological awareness skills of 72 Spanish-English and 86 Cantonese-English bilinguals, all enrolled in bilingual and mainstream classrooms in the same schools. Bilinguals were assessed on phonological awareness, decoding, vocabulary, and knowledge of book reading each year from kindergarten until second grade. Individual growth modeling analysis revealed no difference in growth trajectories of English phonological awareness between (a) Spanish-English and Cantonese-English bilinguals and (b) children enrolled in bilingual and mainstream programs. Within-language decoding, vocabulary, and knowledge of book reading were associated with the estimated average initial levels of phonological awareness skills, while only knowledge of book reading had significant effects on the estimated growth trajectory of phonological awareness skills. These findings suggest that young bilinguals with different home languages may have similar growth trajectories in English phonological awareness skills during early elementary school years. The findings have implications for early educational practices.

Keywords: bilingual; children; language development; longitudinal; phonological awareness.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Average fitted trajectories describing the effect of time-varying English decoding and time-varying English vocabulary on the change in English phonological awareness for Spanish–English and Cantonese–English bilinguals. High is defined as scoring at the 75th percentile, while low is defined as scoring at the 25th percentile (N = 158).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Average fitted trajectories describing the effect of time-varying English decoding and English book reading scores on the change in English phonological awareness for Spanish–English and Cantonese–English bilinguals. High is defined as scoring at the 75th percentile, while low is defined as scoring at the 25th percentile (N = 158).

Similar articles

References

    1. Anthony JL, Lonigan CJ, Driscoll K, Phillips BM, & Burgess SR (2003). Phonological sensitivity: A quasi-parallel progression of word structure units and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 470–487. 10.1598/RRQ.38.4.3 - DOI
    1. Anthony JL, Williams JM, McDonald R, & Francis DJ (2007). Phonological processing and emergent literacy in younger and older preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 113–37. 10.1007/s11881-007-0008-8 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bialystok E, Luk G, & Kwan E (2005). Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 43–61. 10.1207/s1532799xssr0901_4 - DOI
    1. Bialystok E, Majumder S, & Martin MM (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 27–44. 10.1017/S014271640300002X - DOI
    1. Branum-Martin L, Tao S, Garnaat S, Bunta F, & Francis DJ (2012). Meta-analysis of bilingual phonological awareness: Language, age, and psycholinguistic grain size. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 932–944. 10.1037/a0027755 - DOI