Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Sep 23:11:531046.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.531046. eCollection 2020.

Social Feedback During Sensorimotor Synchronization Changes Salivary Oxytocin and Behavioral States

Affiliations

Social Feedback During Sensorimotor Synchronization Changes Salivary Oxytocin and Behavioral States

Claudiu C Papasteri et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

In humans and animal models, oxytocin increases social closeness, attachment and prosocial behaviors, while decreasing anxiety and stress levels. Efficiently triggering the release of endogenous oxytocin could serve as a powerful therapeutic intervention for disorders of social behavior and for anxiety. We designed a new version of a social sensorimotor synchronization task to investigate the role of social approval in inducing biochemical and psychological changes following behavioral synchrony in a sample of 80 college students. Social approval in the form of real time positive feedback increased well-being only in women, while increasing social closeness in both genders. Social disapproval in the form of real time negative feedback prevented a decrease in stress levels that otherwise women reported following engagement in either social or non-social synchronization. Surprisingly, for certain personality traits, negative social feedback during sensorimotor synchronization was psychologically beneficial irrespective of gender. Salivary oxytocin levels increased only in women after the social but not the non-social synchronization tasks. Oxytocin dynamics were independent of the type of real time feedback that subjects received, indicating the existence of distinct mechanisms for hormonal versus behavioral changes following synchronization. Nevertheless, changes in salivary oxytocin after positive social feedback correlated with changes in well-being and predicted changes in prosocial attitudes. Our findings show evidence of distinct mechanisms for behavioral versus hormonal changes following social sensorimotor synchronization, and indicate that gender and personality traits should be carefully considered when designing behavioral therapies for improving social attitudes and for stress management.

Keywords: closeness; gender; oxytocin (OXT); personality traits; social approval; social synchronization; well-being.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Effect of sensoriomotor imitation on endogenous OXT concentrations: (A) “social positive” condition, (B) “non-social” condition, and (C) “social negative” condition. OXT, oxytocin; PRE, before imitation; POST, after imitation. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Gender-specific effect of sensoriomotor imitation on endogenous OXT concentrations: (A) women, “social positive” condition, (B) women, “non-social,” (C) women, “social negative,” (D) men, “social positive,” (E) men, “non-social,” and (F) men, “social negative.” OXT, oxytocin; PRE, before imitation; POST, after imitation. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Effect of sensoriomotor imitation on closeness ratings by gender: (A) women, “social positive” condition, (B) women, “non-social,” (C) women, “social negative,” (D) men, “social positive,” (E) men, “non-social,” (F) men, “social negative.” IOS, inclusion of other in the self-scale**; Pre, before imitation; Post, after imitation. **p < 0.001.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Effect of sensoriomotor imitation on momentary stress levels by gender: (A) women, “social positive” condition, (B) women, “non-social,” (C) women, “social negative,” (D) men, “social positive,” (E) men, “non-social,” (F) men, “social negative.” VAS, visual analog scale; Pre, before imitation; Post, after imitation. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Effect of sensoriomotor imitation on momentary well-being ratings by gender: (A) women, “social positive” condition, (B) women, “non-social,” (C) women, “social negative,” (D) men, “social positive,” (E) men, “non-social,” (F) men, “social negative.” VAS, visual analog scale; Pre, before imitation; Post, after imitation. **p < 0.001.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Investigation of imitation effects on stress changes mediated by modifications in perceived closeness using within-participant mediation model proposed by Yzerbyt et al. (2018). The SEs of point estimates are provided in parentheses. *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Andari E., Hurleman R., Young L. J. (2018). A precision medicine approach to oxytocin trials. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35 559–590. 10.1007/7854_2017_29 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Aoki Y., Yamasue H. (2015). Reply: does imitation act as an oxytocin nebulizer in autism spectrum disorder? Brain 138:e361. 10.1093/brain/awv061 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aoki Y., Yahata N., Watanabe T., Takano Y., Kawakubo Y., Kuwabara H., et al. (2014). Oxytocin improves behavioural and neural deficits in inferring others’ social emotions in autism. Brain 137, 3073–3086. 10.1093/brain/awu231 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aron A., Aron E. N., Smollan D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63 596–612. 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596 - DOI
    1. Ashton-James C., Van Baaren R. B., Chartrand T. L., Decety J., Karremans J. (2007). Mimicry and me: the impact of mimicry on self–construal. Soc. Cogn. 25 518–535. 10.1521/soco.2007.25.4.518 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources