Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Mar 1:282:299-307.
doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.133. Epub 2020 Dec 28.

Recall bias during adolescence: Gender differences and associations with depressive symptoms

Affiliations

Recall bias during adolescence: Gender differences and associations with depressive symptoms

Jessica K Bone et al. J Affect Disord. .

Abstract

Background: There is a sharp increase in depression in females in mid-adolescence, but we do not understand why. Cognitive theories suggest that people with depression have negative biases in recalling self-referential information. We tested whether recall biases were more negative in girls in early and mid-adolescence and were associated with depressive symptoms.

Methods: 315 young and 263 mid-adolescents (11-12 and 13-15 years) completed a surprise test, assessing recall of social evaluation about the self (self-referential) or another person (other-referential). The short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire measured depressive symptoms. We tested the effects of condition (self-referential/other-referential), valence (positive/negative), gender, and age group on correct recall (hits) and associations with depressive symptoms.

Results: There was no evidence for gender or age differences in positive or negative self-referential recall. Self-referential positive hits were negatively associated with depressive symptoms (adjusted coefficient=-0.38, 95% CI=-0.69-0.08, p=0.01). Self-referential negative hits were positively associated with depressive symptoms (adjusted coefficient=0.45, 95% CI=0.15-0.75, p=0.003), and this association was stronger in females (adjusted interaction p=0.04).

Limitations: The reliability and validity of the recall task are unknown. We cannot provide evidence of a causal effect of recall on depressive symptoms in this cross-sectional study.

Conclusions: Adolescents who recalled more self-referential negative and fewer self-referential positive words had more severe depressive symptoms. Females did not demonstrate more recall biases, but the association between self-referential negative hits and depressive symptoms was stronger in females. Negative self-referential recall may be a risk factor for depressive symptoms and is a good candidate for longitudinal studies.

Keywords: Adolescence; Cognition; Depression; Gender difference; Memory; Recall bias.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Social Evaluation Learning task. An example of two trials from a self-referential block, in which the computer character is called Sam and the participant is learning what Sam thinks of them. After viewing a fixation cross, the participant was presented with a positive and negative word pair and instructed to choose the word which best corresponded with what Sam thought about them. They then received feedback about whether their choice was correct (green tick) or incorrect (red cross). Participants used trial and error to learn whether the character liked or disliked them over 20 trials. In the first trial shown here, the participant selected the positive word, which was correct. In the second trial, the participant chose the negative word, which was incorrect. Both of these trials show true (as opposed to misleading) feedback. To prevent ceiling effects, feedback contingency was set at 80%, so that ‘correct’ responses received an 8:2 ratio of positive to negative feedback and ‘incorrect’ responses received an 8:2 ratio of negative to positive feedback.
Figure 2
Figure 2
A) Mean hits according showing three-way interaction between age group, gender, and word condition (self-referential or other-referential). B) Mean hits showing three-way interaction between age group, gender, and word valence (positive or negative). C) Mean hits showing four-way interaction between age group, gender, condition and valence. All plotted using raw data.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alloy LB, Black SK, Young ME, Goldstein KE, Shapero BG, Stange JP, Boccia AS, Matt LM, Boland EM, Moore LC, Abramson LY. Cognitive vulnerabilities and depression versus other psychopathology symptoms and diagnoses in early adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2012;41:539–560. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.703123. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, Pickles A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1995;5:237–249.
    1. Asarnow LD, Thompson RJ, Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Children at risk for depression: Memory biases, self-schemas, and genotypic variation. J Affect Disord. 2014;159:66–72. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Auerbach RP, Bondy E, Stanton CH, Webb CA, Shankman SA, Pizzagalli DA. Self-referential processing in adolescents: Stability of behavioral and ERP markers. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:1398–1406. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12686. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:969–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types