Dogs' insensitivity to scaffolding behaviour in an A-not-B task provides support for the theory of natural pedagogy
- PMID: 33441674
- PMCID: PMC7807054
- DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-79557-8
Dogs' insensitivity to scaffolding behaviour in an A-not-B task provides support for the theory of natural pedagogy
Abstract
Executive function plays a critical role in regulating behaviour. Behaviour which directs attention towards the correct solution leads to increased executive function performance in children, but it is unknown how other animals respond to such scaffolding behaviour. Dogs were presented with an A-not-B detour task. After learning to go through gap A to obtain the reward, the barrier was reversed, and the dogs had to inhibit their learned response and enter through gap B on the opposite side. Failure to do so is known as the perseveration error. In test trials, dogs taking part in one of two scaffolding conditions, a pointing condition, where the experimenter pointed to the new gap, and a demonstration condition, where the experimenter demonstrated the new route, were no less likely to commit the perseveration error than dogs in a control condition with no scaffolding behaviour. Dogs' lack of responsiveness to scaffolding behaviour provides little support for suggestions that simple social learning mechanisms explains scaffolding behaviour in humans. Instead, our results suggest that the theory of natural pedagogy extends to the development of executive function in humans. This suggests that human children's predisposition to interpret ostensive-communicative cues as informative may be an innate, species-specific adaptation.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare no competing interests.
Figures


Similar articles
-
Why do adult dogs (Canis familiaris) commit the A-not-B search error?J Comp Psychol. 2014 Feb;128(1):21-30. doi: 10.1037/a0033084. Epub 2013 Aug 19. J Comp Psychol. 2014. PMID: 23957741
-
You talkin' to me? Functional breed selection may have fundamentally influenced dogs' sensitivity to human verbal communicative cues.BMC Biol. 2024 Aug 26;22(1):183. doi: 10.1186/s12915-024-01983-1. BMC Biol. 2024. PMID: 39183286 Free PMC article.
-
Reassessing shelter dogs' use of human communicative cues in the standard object-choice task.PLoS One. 2019 Mar 7;14(3):e0213166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213166. eCollection 2019. PLoS One. 2019. PMID: 30845205 Free PMC article.
-
Adapting to the human world: dogs' responsiveness to our social cues.Behav Processes. 2009 Mar;80(3):325-33. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.11.002. Epub 2008 Nov 14. Behav Processes. 2009. PMID: 19056474 Review.
-
Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners.Behav Processes. 2009 Jul;81(3):402-8. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.03.017. Behav Processes. 2009. PMID: 19520240 Review.
Cited by
-
Evolution and function of neurocognitive systems in non-human animals.Sci Rep. 2021 Dec 8;11(1):23487. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-02736-8. Sci Rep. 2021. PMID: 34880266 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Diamond, A. The early development of executive functions. In Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change (eds. Bialystok, E. & Craik, F. I. M.) 70–95 (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2006). 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169539.003.0006.
-
- Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M. & Kesek, A. The development of executive function in childhood. In Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (eds. Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M.) 553–574 (The MIT Press, Cambridge, 2008). 10.7551/mitpress/7437.001.0001.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources
Miscellaneous