Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Jan 13;11(1):100.
doi: 10.3390/brainsci11010100.

"My Brain Can Stop": An ERP Study of Longitudinal Prediction of Inhibitory Control in Adolescence

Affiliations

"My Brain Can Stop": An ERP Study of Longitudinal Prediction of Inhibitory Control in Adolescence

Tzlil Einziger et al. Brain Sci. .

Abstract

We examined the longitudinal predictors of electrophysiological and behavioral markers of inhibitory control in adolescence. Participants were 63 adolescent boys who have been followed since birth as part of a prospective longitudinal study on the developmental pathways to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). At 17 years of age, they completed the stop-signal task (SST) while electroencephalography (EEG) was continuously recorded. Inhibitory control was evaluated by the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) as well as by the amplitude of the event-related potential (ERP) component of N2 during successful inhibition. We found that higher inattention symptoms throughout childhood predicted reduced amplitude (i.e., less negative) of the N2 in adolescence. Furthermore, the N2 amplitude was longitudinally predicted by the early precursors of child familial risk for ADHD and early childhood temperament. Specifically, father's inattention symptoms (measured in the child's early infancy) and child's effortful control at 36 months of age directly predicted the N2 amplitude in adolescence, even beyond the consistency of inattention symptoms throughout development. The SSRT was predicted by ADHD symptoms throughout childhood but not by the early precursors. Our findings emphasize the relevance of early familial and temperamental risk for ADHD to the prediction of a later dysfunction in inhibitory control.

Keywords: ADHD; ERP; N2; effortful control; familial risk; inhibitory control.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Grand average event-related potential waveforms of (A) successful stop (B) successful go of slow reaction times and (C) difference wave between a successful stop and successful slow go. Note. The shaded area represents standard error; the gray area delineates the N2 time window; the region of interest is marked with a circle on the electrode map at the top of the figure.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(A) An illustration of the event-related potential wave plot of the difference between a successful stop and successful slow go for adolescents with high and low concurrent inattention symptoms (group defined by the lower and upper quartile). The gray area delineates the N2 time window; the shaded area represents standard error. (B) Topographic maps of voltages on the scalp at the N2 time window (specifically at 200 ms after stop-signal presentation) for high (top) and low (bottom) concurrent inattention symptoms.
Figure 3
Figure 3
A longitudinal pathway from early risk to N2 amplitude at 17 years of age. Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; Standardized estimates of the paths were used; significant paths are represented with solid lines, and insignificant paths are represented with dotted lines.
Figure 4
Figure 4
(A) An event-related potential wave plot of the difference between successful stop and successful slow go for adolescents with high and low ratings of childhood effortful control (group divided into lower and upper quartiles for illustration purpose). The gray area delineates the N2 time window; the shaded area represents standard errors. (B) Topographic maps of voltages on the scalp at the N2 time window (specifically at 200 ms after stop-signal presentation) for high (top) and low (bottom) childhood effortful control. (C) An event-related potential wave plot of the difference between a successful stop and successful slow go for adolescents with high and low paternal inattention symptoms (group defined by the lower and upper quartile). (D) Topographic maps of voltages on the scalp at the N2 time window for high (top) and low (bottom) paternal inattention.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®) American Psychiatric Pub; Washington, DC, USA: 2013.
    1. Polanczyk G.V., Willcutt E.G., Salum G.A., Kieling C., Rohde L.A. ADHD Prevalence Estimates across Three Decades: An Updated Systematic Review and Meta-Regression Analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014;43:434–442. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt261. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Visser S.N., Bitsko R.H., Danielson M.L., Perou R., Blumberg S.J. Increasing Prevalence of Parent-Reported Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder among Children—United States, 2003 and 2007. Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2010;59:1439–1443. - PubMed
    1. Diagnostic A.P.A. Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) APA; Washington, DC, USA: 2013.
    1. Larsson H., Chang Z., D’Onofrio B.M., Lichtenstein P. The Heritability of Clinically Diagnosed Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder across the Life Span. Psychol. Med. 2014;44:2223. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002493. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources