Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Jan 19;16(1):e0245295.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245295. eCollection 2021.

Beyond sensory conflict: The role of beliefs and perception in motion sickness

Affiliations

Beyond sensory conflict: The role of beliefs and perception in motion sickness

Suzanne A E Nooij et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Illusory self-motion often provokes motion sickness, which is commonly explained in terms of an inter-sensory conflict that is not in accordance with previous experience. Here we address the influence of cognition in motion sickness and show that such a conflict is not provocative when the observer believes that the motion illusion is indeed actually occurring. Illusory self-motion and motion sickness were elicited in healthy human participants who were seated on a stationary rotary chair inside a rotating optokinetic drum. Participants knew that both chair and drum could rotate but were unaware of the actual motion stimulus. Results showed that motion sickness was correlated with the discrepancy between participants' perceived self-motion and participants' beliefs about the actual motion. Together with the general motion sickness susceptibility, this discrepancy accounted for 51% of the variance in motion sickness intensity. This finding sheds a new light on the causes of visually induced motion sickness and suggests that it is not governed by an inter-sensory conflict per se, but by beliefs concerning the actual self-motion. This cognitive influence provides a promising tool for the development of new countermeasures.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Experimental set up with motorized chair and drum that could rotate independently from each other.
During the test the surrounding drum was closed and rotating around the stationary chair.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Scatterplots of A) chair rotation likelihood against drum rotation likelihood (n = 19); B) chair rotation likelihood against and Fullvection (i.e., percentage of time spent in full vection, n = 19).
Fig 3
Fig 3. Scatterplots of conflictPB (A) and MSSQ (B) vs. motion sickness intensity (SSQ-TS, n = 19).
Diamonds and circles indicate the sick and non-sick group resp., according to self-judgement.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Mast FW, Preuss N, Hartmann M, Grabherr L. Spatial cognition, body representation and affective processes: the role of vestibular information beyond ocular reflexes and control of posture. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8(44). 10.3389/fnint.2014.00044 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Nigmatullina Y, Arshad Q, Wu K, Seemungal BM, Bronstein AM, Soto D. How imagery changes self-motion perception. Neuroscience. 2015;291:46–52. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.021 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Mertz S, Belkhenchir S, Lepecq JC. Evidence of imagined passive self-motion through imagery-perception interaction. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2000;105(1):57–78. 10.1016/s0001-6918(00)00048-2 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Wertheim AH, Mesland BS, Bles W. Cognitive suppression of tilt sensations during linear horizontal selfmotion in the dark. Perception. 2001;30:733–41. 10.1068/p3092 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ellis AW, Klaus MP, Mast FW. Vestibular cognition: the effect of prior belief on vestibular perceptual decision making. J Neurol. 2017;264:S74–S80. 10.1007/s00415-017-8471-6 - DOI - PubMed