Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Nov;51(11):4213-4226.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04880-6. Epub 2021 Jan 24.

Is a Negative Attentional Bias in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder Explained by Comorbid Depression? An Eye-Tracking Study

Affiliations

Is a Negative Attentional Bias in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder Explained by Comorbid Depression? An Eye-Tracking Study

M Annemiek Bergman et al. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021 Nov.

Abstract

Heightened attention towards negative information is characteristic of depression. Evidence is emerging for a negative attentional bias in Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), perhaps driven by the high comorbidity between ASD and depression. We investigated whether ASD is characterised by a negative attentional bias and whether this can be explained by comorbid (sub) clinical depression. Participants (n = 116) with current (CD) or remitted depression (RD) and/or ASD, and 64 controls viewed positively and negatively valenced (non-)social pictures. Groups were compared on three components of visual attention using linear mixed models. Both CD individuals with and without ASD, but not remitted depressed and never-depressed ASD individuals showed a negative bias, suggesting that negative attentional bias might be a depressive state-specific marker for depression in ASD.

Keywords: Attentional bias; Autism spectrum disorder; Cognitive bias; Comorbidity; Depression.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Left: an example slide from the non-social block, in which two positively valenced pictures (top left and bottom right) and two negatively valenced pictures are depicted (top right and bottom left). Right: an example slide from the social block, in which two positively valenced pictures (top left and bottom right) and two negatively valenced pictures (top right and bottom left) are depicted
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Total gaze duration (ms) per valence (positive vs. negative) for each group, ASD autism spectrum disorder, CD current depression, RD remitted depression, ASD+CD/RD autism spectrum disorder with depression, HC healthy controls, ASD+CD autism spectrum disorder with current depression, ASD+RD autism spectrum disorder with remitted depression. Asterisks highlight significantly different mean comparisons (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001). Error bars represent standard errors

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®) Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.
    1. Armstrong T, Olatunji BO. Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2012;32(8):704–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31(1):5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bauminger N, Shulman C, Agam G. Peer interaction and loneliness in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33(5):489–507. doi: 10.1023/A:1025827427901. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baxter AJ, Brugha T, Erskine H, Scheurer R, Vos T, Scott J. The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(3):601–613. doi: 10.1017/S003329171400172X. - DOI - PubMed