Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Mar 19:7:26.
doi: 10.3389/frobt.2020.00026. eCollection 2020.

Re-association of Body Parts: Illusory Ownership of a Virtual Arm Associated With the Contralateral Real Finger by Visuo-Motor Synchrony

Affiliations

Re-association of Body Parts: Illusory Ownership of a Virtual Arm Associated With the Contralateral Real Finger by Visuo-Motor Synchrony

Ryota Kondo et al. Front Robot AI. .

Abstract

Illusory ownership can be induced in a virtual body by visuo-motor synchrony. Our aim was to test the possibility of a re-association of the right thumb with a virtual left arm and express the illusory body ownership of the re-associated arm through a synchronous or asynchronous movement of the body parts through action and vision. Participants felt that their right thumb was the virtual left arm more strongly in the synchronous condition than in the asynchronous one, and the feeling of ownership of the virtual arm was also stronger in the synchronous condition. We did not find a significant difference in the startle responses to a sudden knife appearance to the virtual arm between the two synchrony conditions, as there was no proprioceptive drift of the thumb. These results suggest that a re-association of the right thumb with the virtual left arm could be induced by visuo-motor synchronization; however, it may be weaker than the natural association.

Keywords: illusory body ownership; re-association; sense of agency; virtual reality; visuo-motor synchrony.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Apparatus.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Joint correspondence of the thumb and the virtual arm.
Figure 3
Figure 3
A virtual left arm and a virtual knife.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Self-localization task.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Results of the questionnaire. The error bars indicate standard errors (SE). *, ** indicate statistical significance at the 0.05 (p < 0.05) and 0.01 (p < 0.01) levels with Wilcoxon signed-rank test, respectively.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Result of startle response (mean). The error bars indicate SE.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Results of self-localization drift (A) and perceived length of the finger (B).

References

    1. Aglioti S., Bonazzi A., Cortese F. (1994). Phantom lower limb as a perceptual marker of neural plasticity in the mature human brain. Proc. Biol. Sci. 255, 273–278. 10.1098/rspb.1994.0039 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aglioti S., Smania N., Atzei A., Berlucchi G. (1997). Spatio-temporal properties of the pattern of evoked phantom sensations in a left index amputee patient. Behav. Neurosci. 111:867–872. 10.1037/0735-7044.111.5.867 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Antfolk C., D'alonzo M., Rosen B., Lundborg G., Sebelius F., Cipriani C. (2013). Sensory feedback in upper limb prosthetics. Exp. Rev. Med. Devices 10, 45–54. 10.1586/erd.12.68 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Banakou D., Groten R., Slater M. (2013). Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 12846–12851. 10.1073/pnas.1306779110 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Blanke O., Metzinger T. (2009). Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 7–13. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources