Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Feb;6(2):e106-e115.
doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30258-9.

Sex differences and the role of education in cognitive ageing: analysis of two UK-based prospective cohort studies

Affiliations

Sex differences and the role of education in cognitive ageing: analysis of two UK-based prospective cohort studies

Mikaela Bloomberg et al. Lancet Public Health. 2021 Feb.

Abstract

Background: Previous studies have shown an excess risk of Alzheimer's disease and related dementias among women. Education is thought to have a causal association with dementia onset. We aimed to investigate the role of education in influencing sex differences in cognitive ageing.

Methods: We analysed data from two prospective cohort studies in the UK; the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) and the Whitehall II study, to assess sex differences in cognitive performance and cognitive decline by birth cohort (birth year 1930-38, 1939-45, or 1946-55), before and after adjustment for education, and by high and low education level. Memory was assessed using immediate recall, for which data were available from all waves of the ELSA (2002-14) and Whitehall II (1997-2015) studies. Fluency was assessed using a semantic fluency test based on an animal naming task, with data available from all waves of the Whitehall II study and waves one to five (2002-10) and wave seven (2014) of the ELSA study. Cognitive scores were standardised separately in each study based on the mean and SD of the corresponding test among participants aged 50-59 years with secondary education.

Findings: 15 924 participants were included from the two studies. In pooled analyses, women had better memory scores than men in all birth cohorts, irrespective of adjustment for education (eg, at age 60 years, birth cohort 1930-38, mean difference between sexes [male scores minus female scores] -0·25 SDs [95% CI -0·32 to -0·19] after adjustment for education), and in both education level groups. Memory decline was faster in men than in women (at age 60 years, birth cohort 1946-55, mean difference in 13-year change -0·15 SDs [-0·20 to -0·09]; after adjustment for education -0·14 SDs [-0·20 to -0·08]). Men had better fluency scores than women in earlier birth cohorts and in the low education group (at age 60 years, birth cohort 1930-38, mean difference 0·20 SDs [95% CI 0·05 to 0·36]); but women had better fluency scores than men in later birth cohorts and in the high education group (at age 60 years, birth cohort 1946-55, mean difference -0·17 SDs [-0·24 to -0·10]). No sex differences were observed for fluency decline.

Interpretation: Our findings suggest that decreasing disparities between sexes in education, due to secular increases in educational opportunities, could attenuate sex differences in dementia risk and cognitive decline in the future.

Funding: National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health; UK Medical Research Council; British Heart Foundation; and National Institute for Health Research.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Cognitive performance trajectories before and after adjustment for education Base models adjusted for ethnicity, practice effect, and interactions with age. Results are shown for ethnicity (white) and practice effect (no practice effect) reference categories. Model adjusted for education, and interactions with age is shown for the education reference category (no qualifications).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Cognitive performance trajectories stratified by education Adjusted for ethnicity, practice effect, and interactions with age. Results are shown for ethnicity (white) and practice effect (no practice effect) reference categories. Low education was defined as qualifications below A-level; high education was A-level qualifications or higher.

Comment in

  • Dementia risk: time matters.
    Selbæk G. Selbæk G. Lancet Public Health. 2021 Feb;6(2):e85-e86. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00010-4. Lancet Public Health. 2021. PMID: 33516290 No abstract available.

References

    1. Hay SI, Abajobir AA, Abate KH. Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life-years (DALYs) for 333 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet. 2017;390:1260–1344. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Podcasy JL, Epperson CN. Considering sex and gender in Alzheimer disease and other dementias. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18:437–446. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Mielke MM, Vemuri P, Rocca WA. Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer's disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin Epidemiol. 2014;6:37–48. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Gao S, Hendrie HC, Hall KS, Hui S. The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:809–815. - PubMed
    1. Nebel RA, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL. Understanding the impact of sex and gender in Alzheimer's disease: a call to action. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:1171–1183. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types