Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Mar 26;10(3):bio058649.
doi: 10.1242/bio.058649.

Spatial odor discrimination in the hawkmoth, Manduca sexta (L.)

Affiliations

Spatial odor discrimination in the hawkmoth, Manduca sexta (L.)

Kalyanasundaram Parthasarathy et al. Biol Open. .

Abstract

Flying insects track turbulent odor plumes to find mates, food and egg-laying sites. To maintain contact with the plume, insects are thought to adapt their flight control according to the distribution of odor in the plume using the timing of odor onsets and intervals between odor encounters. Although timing cues are important, few studies have addressed whether insects are capable of deriving spatial information about odor distribution from bilateral comparisons between their antennae in flight. The proboscis extension reflex (PER) associative learning protocol, originally developed to study odor learning in honeybees, was used as a tool to ask if hawkmoths, Manduca sexta, can discriminate between odor stimuli arriving on either antenna. We show moths discriminated the odor arrival side with an accuracy of >70%. Information about spatial distribution of odor stimuli may be available to moths searching for odor sources, opening the possibility that they use both spatial and temporal odor information.This article has an associated First Person interview with the first author of the paper.

Keywords: Odor localization; Olfaction.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing interestsThe authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Behavioral setup and training protocol. (A) Schematic diagram of behavioral setup showing a restrained moth with its straightened proboscis and odor delivery onto its antennae. Reward delivery port was placed at the distal tip of the proboscis. The feeding response was monitored using EMG activity of the suction pump muscles in the head. (B) Schematic diagram shows events during training and test protocols. Odor was presented for 5 s and 3 s after odor onset the reward valve was activated for 0.1 s. Moths were not rewarded during test session.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Odor-arrival side discrimination response. Pie charts show the number of moths that learned the odor- arrival side task for both linalool (A) and benzaldehyde (D). Bar graphs showing EMG % response for both left (blue bar) and right (red bar) side odor delivery. Panels B and E show the moths’ response when their left antenna was conditioned, and panels C and F when their right antenna was conditioned. (B,C) Moths discriminated linalool arrival side. Response to the associated side was higher, >70% compared to the unassociated side, <30%. (C) Moths discriminated benzaldehyde arrival side. Response to the associated side was higher, >70% compared to the unassociated side, <25%. (B,C,E and F) Error bars indicate standard error of mean.

References

    1. Ando, N. and Kanzaki, R. (2015). A simple behaviour provides accuracy and flexibility in odour plume tracking - the robotic control of sensory-motor coupling in silkmoths. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 3845-3854. 10.1242/jeb.124834 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Arbas, E. A., Willis, M. A. and Kanzaki, R. (1993). Organization of goal-oriented locomotion: pheromone-modulated flight behavior of moths. In Biological Neural Networks in Invertebrate Neuroethology and Robotics. (ed. Beer R. D., Ritzmann R. E. and McKenna T. M.), pp. 159-198. Academic Press.
    1. Baker, C. F. and Montgomery, J. C. (1999). The sensory basis of rheotaxis in the blind Mexican cave fish, Astyanax fasciatus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 184, 519-527. 10.1007/s003590050351 - DOI
    1. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1-48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 - DOI
    1. Bisch-Knaden, S., Dahake, A., Sachse, S., Knaden, M. and Hansson, B. S. (2018). Spatial representation of feeding and oviposition odors in the brain of a hawkmoth. Cell Rep. 22, 2482-2492. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.01.082 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources