Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 May;26(2):679-696.
doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12519. Epub 2021 Mar 24.

Compliance without fear: Individual-level protective behaviour during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic

Affiliations

Compliance without fear: Individual-level protective behaviour during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic

Frederik Jørgensen et al. Br J Health Psychol. 2021 May.

Abstract

Objectives: The outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic required rapid public compliance with advice from health authorities. Here, we ask who was most likely to do so during the first wave of the pandemic.

Design: Quota-sampled cross-sectional and panel data from eight Western democracies (Denmark, France, Germany, Hungary, Italy, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States).

Methods: We fielded online public opinion surveys to 26,508 citizens between 19 March and 16 May. The surveys included questions about protective behaviour, perceptions of the pandemic (threat and self-efficacy), as well as broader attitudes towards society (institutional and interpersonal trust). We employ multilevel and fixed-effects regression models to analyse the relationship between these variables.

Results: Consistent with prior research on epidemics, perceptions of threat turn out as culturally uniform determinants of both avoidant and preventive forms of protective behaviour. On this basis, authorities could foster compliance by appealing to fear of COVID-19, but there may be normative and practical limits to such a strategy. Instead, we find that another major source of compliance is a sense of self-efficacy. Using individual-level panel data, we find evidence that self-efficacy is amendable to change and exerts an effect on protective behaviour. Furthermore, the effects of fear are small among those who feel efficacious, creating a path to compliance without fear. In contrast, two other major candidates for facilitating compliance from the social sciences, interpersonal trust and institutional trust, have surprisingly little motivational power during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic.

Conclusions: To address future waves of the pandemic, health authorities should thus focus on facilitating self-efficacy in the public.

Keywords: COVID-19; efficacy; fear; protective behaviour; trust.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

All authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Selection of countries for data collection. Note: Solid black (avoidant behaviour) and grey (preventive) lines are the developments in the present measures of self‐reported protective behaviour. Dashed lines are the developments in policy stringency (Hale et al.,2020). Grey bars display the developments in the COVID‐19 case counts per capita.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Correlations between protective behaviour and psychological correlates. Note: Correlations from our benchmark model. Filled black circles (grey triangles) show the estimated country‐specific correlations between each of the psychological variables and avoidant behaviour (preventive behaviour). Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Country‐specific correlations between protective behaviour and psychological correlates. Note: Filled black circles (grey triangles) show the estimated country‐specific correlations between each of our psychological variables and avoidant behaviour (preventive behaviour). Black (grey) dashed lines refer to the estimated overall associations in Figure 2. Error bars are 95 % confidence intervals that show whether the country‐specific correlations are statistically significantly different from overall associations
Figure 4
Figure 4
Do self‐efficacy and trust moderate the correlation between worry and protective behaviour? Note: Solid black lines show predicted values at high levels of each moderator, black dashed lines show predicted values at medium levels, and solid grey lines show predicted values at low levels
Figure 5
Figure 5
Country‐specific moderations. Note: Filled black circles (grey triangles) show the estimated country‐specific moderations. Error bars are 95 % confidence intervals that show whether the country‐specific moderations are statistically significantly different from overall moderations (see Figure 4)

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aarøe L., Osmundsen M., Petersen M. B. (2016). Distrust As a Disease Avoidance Strategy: Individual Differences in Disgust Sensitivity Regulate Generalized Social Trust. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–14. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01038 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Amidon, T. R. , Nielsen, A. C. , Pugfelder, E. H. , Richards, D. P. , & Stephens, S. H. (2021). Visual risk literacy in “flatten the curve” covid‐19 visualizations. Journal of Business and Technical Communication, 35(1), 101–109. 10.1177/1050651920963439 - DOI
    1. Anderson, R. M. , Heesterbeek, H. , Klinkenberg, D. , & Hollingsworth, T. D. (2020). How will country‐based mitigation measures influence the course of the covid‐19 epidemic? The Lancet, 395, 931–934. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30567-5 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Angrist, J. D. , & Pischke, J.‐S. (2008). Mostly harmless econometrics: An empiricist's companion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
    1. Bandura, A. (1982). Self‐efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147. 10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122 - DOI

Publication types