Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Sep;6(3):303-308.
doi: 10.1037/mot0000159. Epub 2019 Oct 3.

Effort in daily life: relationships between experimental tasks and daily experience

Affiliations

Effort in daily life: relationships between experimental tasks and daily experience

Adam J Culbreth et al. Motiv Sci. 2020 Sep.

Abstract

Recently, experimental tasks have been developed which index individual differences in willingness to expend effort for reward. However, little is known regarding whether such measures are associated with daily experience of effort. To test this, 31 participants completed an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) protocol, answering surveys regarding the mental and physical demand of their daily activities, and also completed two effort-based decision-making tasks: the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT) and the Cognitive Effort Discounting (COGED) Task. Individuals who reported engaging in more mentally and physically demanding activities via EMA were also more willing to expend effort in the COGED task. However, EMA variables were not significantly associated with EEfRT decision-making. The results demonstrate the ecological, discriminant, and incremental validity of the COGED task, and provide preliminary evidence that individual differences in daily experience of effort may arise, in part, from differences in trait-level tendencies to weigh the costs versus benefits of actions.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1:
Figure 1:
Frequency of Self-Reported Activities during the EMA Protocol
Figure 2:
Figure 2:. Effort-Based Decision-Making Task Performance:
Top (EEfRT): individuals selected the hard task with greater frequency as reward value and probability of reward receipt increased, but we did not find a significant interaction reward by probability interaction. Bottom (COGED): subjective values decreased with higher N-back levels (i.e., they were discounted more steeply with increasing N-back load) However, subjective value did not vary by hard task offer amount (i.e., participants discounted $2 and $4 offer values similarly).
Figure 3:
Figure 3:
Associations between Effort-Based Decision-Making and Self-Reported Daily Effort Comment: Shaded region indicates 95% confidence interval

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Barch DM, Treadway MT, & Schoen N (2014). Effort, anhedonia, and function in schizophrenia: Reduced effort allocation predicts amotivation and functional impairment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 387–397. 10.1037/a0036299 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, & Tily HJ (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bates D, & Sarkar D (2007). Ime4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. ‘R’package Version 0.9975–12. URL http://CRAN.R-project.org.
    1. Brehm JW, & Self EA (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40(1), 109–131. 10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Cacioppo JT, & Petty RE (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131. 10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116 - DOI