Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Observational Study
. 2021 Apr 9;12(1):2134.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-22443-2.

Varying demands for cognitive control reveals shared neural processes supporting semantic and episodic memory retrieval

Affiliations
Observational Study

Varying demands for cognitive control reveals shared neural processes supporting semantic and episodic memory retrieval

Deniz Vatansever et al. Nat Commun. .

Abstract

The categorisation of long-term memory into semantic and episodic systems has been an influential catalyst for research on human memory organisation. However, the impact of variable cognitive control demands on this classical distinction remains to be elucidated. Across two independent experiments, here we directly compare neural processes for the controlled versus automatic retrieval of semantic and episodic memory. In a multi-session functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment, we first identify a common cluster of cortical activity centred on the left inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insular cortex for the retrieval of both weakly-associated semantic and weakly-encoded episodic memory traces. In an independent large-scale individual difference study, we further reveal a common neural circuitry in which reduced functional interaction between the identified cluster and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, a default mode network hub, is linked to better performance across both memory types. Our results provide evidence for shared neural processes supporting the controlled retrieval of information from functionally distinct long-term memory systems.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1. Experimental design and behavioural performance during long-term memory retrieval.
a On day one, a group of healthy young adult participants completed a 3-AFC semantic memory retrieval fMRI task in which they were probed with a word (e.g., bee) and asked to select the most conceptually associated target word (e.g., string). On a separate day, participants were trained on pairs of conceptually unrelated words (e.g., apple—flute) using both passive and active encoding. The next day, participants attended an fMRI scanning session in which they were tested during a 3-AFC episodic memory retrieval fMRI task with the same parameters employed for the semantic task. The strength of conceptual associations in the semantic task and level of encoding practice in the episodic task were manipulated (strong versus weak trials). b Overall, participants performed better in the retrieval of strongly as compared to weakly associated word pairs for both the semantic and the episodic 3-AFC fMRI tasks. There was also a significant difference in memory type with participants performing better in the episodic in comparison with the semantic memory retrieval task (Supplementary Notes S1). The violin plots illustrate a boxplot with the median (centre white dot), the interquartile range (black bar), the minima/maxima values (thin black line) as well as the kernel density estimation of the underlying distribution. *** denotes p < 0.001 in paired t-tests corrected for multiple comparisons. n = 46 independent participants examined over two paired fMRI tasks. Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2. Differential brain activity patterns in the retrieval of long-term memory types.
a The comparison of semantic to episodic memory retrieval, across all trial types versus control trials, revealed greater activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG). b The reverse contrast on the other hand, illustrated greater activity centred on the posterior cingulate/precuneal cortices (PCC/PCUN) and bilateral angular gyri (AG) extending towards the lateral occipital cortex. c, d These results spatially overlapped with meta-analytic difference maps obtained from the subtraction of association maps linked to the cognitive terms semantic (n = 1031) versus episodic (n = 488) in the Neurosynth database (aTL = anterior temporal lobe, pMTG = posterior middle temporal gyrus, SFG = superior frontal gyrus, RSC = retrosplenial cortex, MTL = medial temporal lobe). All task-fMRI results were corrected for general task difficulty effects using inverse efficiency scores across memory tasks as covariates of no interest.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3. Shared brain activity patterns associated with controlled retrieval across long-term memory types.
a For both semantic and episodic 3-AFC fMRI tasks, shared neural responses were observed in the retrieval of weakly versus strongly associated word-pairs. b A formal conjunction analysis illustrated that for the weak > strong contrast, left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and anterior insular cortex (aINS) showed greater activity for both memory types, which largely overlapped with the frontoparietal, salience/ventral attention, and default mode networks (based on the Yeo 7-Network parcellation) (VN = visual network, SMN = somatomotor network, DAN = dorsal attention network, S/VAN = salience/ventral attention network, LN = limbic network, FPN = frontoparietal network, DMN = default mode network). c, d The peristimulus time plots (for illustrative purposes only) showed sustained activity differences between weak versus strong trials (in comparison to implicit baseline) that spanned 1-3 repetition times (TRs) across all participants for both memory types. While curved lines represent the best fit to a smoothing spline over mean activity values, shaded areas reflect 95% confidence intervals. e LIFG/aINS activity differences during weak versus control trials between semantic and episodic memory tasks were significantly correlated across participants (partial rp = .29, p = 0.036, correcting for age and gender). Straight-line represents the best fit, while shaded areas illustrate 95% confidence intervals. No such correlation was observed for activity differences between strong versus control trials for this cluster of brain regions across the two memory types (partial rp = .11, p = .24, correcting for age and gender). Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4. Common neural circuitry linked to the controlled retrieval of semantic and episodic memory.
a A large cohort of 140 participants underwent a nine-minute long resting-state fMRI scanning. In the analysis of this Experiment 2 dataset, the significant cluster that was linked to the retrieval of weak versus strong associations across both memory types in Experiment 1, was used as the seed region-of-interest in a functional connectivity analysis. Across participants, lower positive connectivity (greater anti-correlation) of the left inferior frontal gyrus/anterior insular cortex (LIFG/aINS) cluster to the bilateral ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) was related to better performance in the retrieval of both weakly associated semantic and episodic word pairs, measured outside the scanner (corrected for age, gender and head-motion). The scatter plot is shown for illustrative purposes only. While the straight lines represent the best linear fit over individual values, shaded areas illustrate 95% confidence intervals. Individual variation in this neural circuitry was not related to fluid intelligence (partial rp = −0.010, p = 0.45), or selective attention / inhibitory control (partial rp = 0.052, p = 0.27). n = 140 independent participants examined over one experiment. b The unthresholded results from this regression model were meta-analytically decoded for cognitive terms using the Neurosynth database. The top 100 terms are presented in a word cloud in which text size corresponds to the strength of association (anatomical terms were removed). c In a separate seed-based functional connectivity analysis, the positive connectivity (r > 0) of the vmPFC revealed large overlaps with the default mode and limbic networks, whereas negative connectivity (r < 0) was characterized by salience/ventral/dorsal attention and frontoparietal networks, based on the Yeo 7-Network parcellation scheme (VN = visual network, SMN = somatomotor network, DAN = dorsal attention network, S/VAN = salience/ventral attention network, LN = limbic network, FPN = frontoparietal network, DMN = default mode network). Source data are provided as a Source Data file.

References

    1. Tulving E., Donaldson W., Bower G. H., United States. Office of Naval Research. Organization of memory. Academic Press (1972).
    1. Eichenbaum H. Memory: organization and control. Annu Rev. Psychol. 2017;68:19–45. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044131. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Patterson K, Nestor PJ, Rogers TT. Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:976–987. doi: 10.1038/nrn2277. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Dickerson BC, Eichenbaum H. The episodic memory system: neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:86–104. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.126. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex. 2009;19:2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types