Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Mar 15:14:100767.
doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100767. eCollection 2021 Jun.

Adverse childhood circumstances and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Chinese adults: Lower level or faster decline?

Affiliations

Adverse childhood circumstances and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Chinese adults: Lower level or faster decline?

Zhuoer Lin et al. SSM Popul Health. .

Abstract

We examine the long-term relationship between childhood circumstances and cognitive aging. In particular, we differentiate the level of cognitive deficit from the rate of cognitive decline. Applying a linear mixed-effect model to three waves of China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Surveys (CHARLS 2011, 2013, 2015) and matching cognitive outcomes to CHARLS Life History Survey (2014), we find that key domains of childhood circumstances, including family socioeconomic status (SES), neighborhood cohesion, friendship, and health conditions, are significantly associated with both the level of cognitive deficit and the rate of decline. In contrast, childhood neighborhood safety only affects the level of cognitive deficit. Childhood relationship with mother only affects the rate of cognitive decline. The effects of adverse childhood circumstances are generally larger on level of cognitive deficit than on rate of cognitive decline. Moreover, education plays a more important role in mediating the relationships compared to other later-life factors. These findings suggest that exposure to disadvantaged childhood circumstances can exacerbate cognitive deficit as well as cognitive decline over time, which may be partially ameliorated by educational attainment.

Keywords: Childhood circumstances; Cognitive aging; Education; Life course factors.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Course of cognitive aging with diverse childhood circumstances. Notes: Panel A–J illustrate the diverse course of cognitive aging from age 45 to age 80 with different childhood circumstances, including parental education, parental work status, childhood relationships, childhood neighborhood social environment, and childhood health conditions. The X axis denotes the respondents’ age when their cognitions were assessed. As cognition function is measured longitudinally in CHARLS, each individual may contribute more than one observation to the trend, and his/her cognitive function may reflect in more than one age group depending on the exact time of cognitive assessment. The plotted points in each panel, thus, represent the average level of cognitive deficit, for the ones with particular ages when the cognitive test was conducted, and with particular childhood circumstances. Cognitive deficit is defined as the reversed summary score of five cognitive tests, with higher value indicating greater cognitive deficit; age is specified as the age when the cognitive tests were conducted. All the regression lines are fitted using local linear smoothing.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
The effects of childhood circumstances on cognitive aging among people with lower and higher education. Panel A. Childhood circumstances and level of cognitive deficit by education.Panel B. Childhood circumstances and rate of cognitive decline by education. Notes: Coefficient plots of the childhood circumstances on level of cognitive deficit (Panel A) and rate of decline (Panel B) among people with lower (primary school or below) and higher education level (middle school or above). The cross-equation test is conducted respectively to examine the statistical difference between the coefficients in two linear regressions. P-value is calculated based on Chow/Wald test, showing at the rightmost side of each panel (Only significant results are illustrated, whereas other estimates are available upon request. Statistical significance: ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Almond D., Mazumder B. Health capital and the prenatal environment: The effect of ramadan observance during pregnancy. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2011;3:56–85. doi: 10.1257/app.3.4.56. - DOI
    1. Aneshensel C.S., Sucoff C.A. The neighborhood context of adolescent mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1996;37:293–310. doi: 10.2307/2137258. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Attanasio O.P., Meghir C., Santiago A. Education choices in Mexico: Using a structural model and a randomized experiment to evaluate progresa. The Review of Economic Studies. 2012;79:37–66.
    1. Barker D.J.P. The developmental origins of adult disease. Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2004;23:588S–595S. - PubMed
    1. Barnes L.L., Wilson R.S., Everson-Rose S.A., Hayward M.D., Evans D.A., Mendes de Leon C.F. Effects of early-life adversity on cognitive decline in older African Americans and whites. Neurology. 2012;79:2321–2327. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318278b607. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources