Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Apr 15;16(4):e0249111.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249111. eCollection 2021.

Neural correlates of confusability in recognition of morphologically complex Korean words

Affiliations

Neural correlates of confusability in recognition of morphologically complex Korean words

Jeahong Kim et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

When people confuse and reject a non-word that is created by switching two adjacent letters from an actual word, is called the transposition confusability effect (TCE). The TCE is known to occur at the very early stages of visual word recognition with such unit exchange as letters or syllables, but little is known about the brain mechanisms of TCE. In this study, we examined the neural correlates of TCE and the effect of a morpheme boundary placement on TCE. We manipulated the placement of a morpheme boundary by exchanging places of two syllables embedded in Korean morphologically complex words made up of lexical morpheme and grammatical morpheme. In the two experimental conditions, the transposition syllable within-boundary condition (TSW) involved exchanging two syllables within the same morpheme, whereas the across-boundary condition (TSA) involved the exchange of syllables across the stem and grammatical morpheme boundary. During fMRI, participants performed the lexical decision task. Behavioral results revealed that the TCE was found in TSW condition, and the morpheme boundary, which is manipulated in TSA, modulated the TCE. In the fMRI results, TCE induced activation in the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). The IPS activation was specific to a TCE and its strength of activation was associated with task performance. Furthermore, two functional networks were involved in the TCE: the central executive network and the dorsal attention network. Morpheme boundary modulation suppressed the TCE by recruiting the prefrontal and temporal regions, which are the key regions involved in semantic processing. Our findings propose the role of the dorsal visual pathway in syllable position processing and that its interaction with other higher cognitive systems is modulated by the morphological boundary in the early phases of visual word recognition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Experimental design.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Behavioral results of reaction time (RT) and error rate for TSA, TSW, and RS conditions.
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
Fig 3
Fig 3. The results of whole brain analysis.
Red color indicates TSA > Baseline, green color indicates TSW > Baseline, and yellow color indicates overlapping brain areas. p < .05 FDR-corrected at a cluster level, p < .001, Ks > 30 at a voxel level.
Fig 4
Fig 4. fMRI analysis of the TSA and TSW contrasts with RS.
(a) The whole brain activation map of TSA > RS (up: mPFC, down: Left mMTG); (b) whole brain activation of TSW > RS (left IPL); (c) whole brain activation of TSW > TSA (left IPS). Left column shows whole brain activation map, middle column gives ROI results, and right column presents a correlation plot between beta value and behavioral data (reaction time). ** p < .01.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Functional connectivity results.
Red indicates TSA condition, green indicates TSW condition, and yellow indicates overlapping. *** p < 0.001, * p < 0.05.
Fig 6
Fig 6. The results of the FC between the IPL and orthographic regions (pFFG, pMOG, and aFFG) and semantic area (pMTG) during the TSW.
* p < .05.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Chambers SM. Letter and order information in lexical access. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1979; 18(2):225–241.
    1. Frankish C, Turner E. SIHGT and SUNOD: The role of orthography and phonology in the perception of transposed letter anagrams. Journal of Memory and Language. 2007; 56(2):189–211.
    1. O’Connor RE, Forster KI. Criterion bias and search sequence bias in word recognition. Memory & Cognition. 1981; 9(1):78–92. 10.3758/bf03196953 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Perea M, Carreiras M. Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a syllable level? Experimental Psychology. 2006; 53(4):308–315. 10.1027/1618-3169.53.4.308 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Taft M, Nillsen C. Morphological decomposition and the transposed-letter (TL) position effect. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2013; 28(7):917–938.

Publication types