Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Apr 19:10:e61942.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.61942.

Spontaneous and evoked activity patterns diverge over development

Affiliations

Spontaneous and evoked activity patterns diverge over development

Lilach Avitan et al. Elife. .

Abstract

The immature brain is highly spontaneously active. Over development this activity must be integrated with emerging patterns of stimulus-evoked activity, but little is known about how this occurs. Here we investigated this question by recording spontaneous and evoked neural activity in the larval zebrafish tectum from 4 to 15 days post-fertilisation. Correlations within spontaneous and evoked activity epochs were comparable over development, and their neural assemblies refined in similar ways. However, both the similarity between evoked and spontaneous assemblies, and also the geometric distance between spontaneous and evoked patterns, decreased over development. At all stages of development, evoked activity was of higher dimension than spontaneous activity. Thus, spontaneous and evoked activity do not converge over development in this system, and these results do not support the hypothesis that spontaneous activity evolves to form a Bayesian prior for evoked activity.

Keywords: neural coding; neural development; neuroscience; spontaneous activity; zebrafish.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

LA, ZP, JM, SZ, BS, GG No competing interests declared

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Population activity during both spontaneous and evoked activity has similar correlational structure.
(A) Top: Larvae were embedded in agarose with one eye facing the projected image for two-photon calcium imaging. Bottom: The contralateral optic tectum (in this example 15 dpf) was imaged for 96.6 min. The neuropil (NP) contour of each fish was fitted with an ellipse (dashed line) with the major axis defining the tectal anterior-posterior axis (AP axis). Periventricular layer (PVL), NP, anterior (A) and posterior (P) ends of the tectum are indicated. (B) Experimental protocol. Tectal spontaneous activity (SA) in the dark was recorded after which fish were exposed to light and given 5 min to adjust. We then recorded evoked activity (TEA) in response to 20 trials of the stimulus set consisting of spots at positions 45°, 60°, 75°, 90°, 105°, 120°, 135°, 150°, 165° of the visual field (where 0° was defined as the body axis), presented in an order which maximised spatial separation within a trial. The inter-trial interval was 25 s. (C) Raster plot for an example 15 dpf fish showing concerted neural activity during 8 min of spontaneous activity in the dark and then during three cycles of stimulus presentation (stimulus onset is marked by black triangles). (D) Short-range pairwise correlation coefficients were higher for TEA compared to SA (4 dpf: p=10-4 for up to 50 µm, p=10-3 for 50–100 µm; 5 dpf: p=10-3 for up to 50 µm, p=10-3 for 50–100 µm; 15 dpf: p=10-3 for up to 50 µm, p=10-2 for 50–100 µm). (E) TEA and SA correlation matrices showed structural similarity (example shown is for a 15 dpf fish). Neurons were sorted by their position on the AP axis. (F) Correlation between TEA and SA correlation matrices does not change over development (one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni multiple comparison correction).
Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
Figure 1—figure supplement 1.. Number of active neurons recorded was similar at different ages.
One-way ANOVA, p=0.4.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Stimulus driven and spontaneous recurring assemblies undergo similar global circuitry effects.
(A) Nine total evoked activity (TEA) assemblies (black) and three spontaneous activity (SA) assemblies (gray) for the 15 dpf fish shown in Figure 1A. The outline shows the periventricular layer (PVL) within the field of view, with each dot representing a neuron. (B) Number of assembly neurons assigned to TEA or SA assemblies did not change over development (t-test 4 vs. 15 TEA p=0.5; SA p=0.8). (C-D) Centres of mass (CoMs) of assemblies within TEA or SA shifted posteriorly over development. Each column of points represents assembly CoMs for a single fish. E-F: Assembly neurons’ fitted tuning curves of the most anterior (E) and posterior (F) TEA assemblies from A (inset) illustrate tuning variability within the assembly. (G) Mean assembly tuning of TEA (filled circles) and SA (empty circles) assemblies shows a posterior shift in tuning with age. Dashed line represents the perfectly linear mapping. (H) Tuning variance is higher for SA assemblies than TEA assemblies (t-test), indicating that in general tuning of spontaneous assemblies is more dispersed than stimulus driven assemblies. I: Mean overlap between TEA and SA assemblies decreases between 4 and 5 dpf and remains stable over development.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.. Tuning properties of neural assemblies over development.
(A) The area covered by the smallest polygon bounding the assembly shows no developmental trend (one-way ANOVA). (B) The area covered by spontaneous activity (SA) assemblies is larger than the area covered by evoked activity (TEA) assemblies (t-test). (C) Assembly tuning variance shows no developmental trend (one-way ANOVA, p=0.66, p=0.62 for SA and TEA respectively). (D) Tuning variance over the anterior-posterior (AP) axis shows a decrease in tuning variance as a function of the assembly centre of mass (CoM). Linear regression EA: p=10-4 for 4 dpf, p=10-4 for 5 dpf, p=10-4 for 9 dpf, p=10-4 for 15 dpf; SA: p=10-2 for 4 dpf, p=10-3 for 5 dpf, p=0.03 for 9 dpf, p=0.09 for 15 dpf.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Dimensionality of evoked activity is higher than that of spontaneous activity.
(A) Example showing the number of coactive neurons during 8 min of spontaneous activity in the dark and then during three cycles of stimulus presentation (stimulus onset is marked by black triangles). Significant peaks of coactivity levels (p<0.05, shuffled SA) during SA, EA (black dots), SE (open circles) are marked. (B) There are higher levels of mean coactivity during EA compared to SE and SA (one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni multiple comparison correction). (C) Number of principal components required to explain 80% of the variance (one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni multiple comparison correction) indicates higher dimensionality for evoked responses. In addition to the comparison shown, the dimensionality within the EA epoch increased over development; 4 (dpf) vs. 15 dpf p=0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni multiple comparison correction. (D) Pattern similarity decreased between all pairs of epochs over development. Similarity was defined as the cosine similarity between the different epochs. (E) Schematic of activity patterns geometry. The patterns of a particular epoch, in this example SA patterns, were collected (red dots), and a linear subspace spanned by these patterns was calculated (HSA, red plane; in reality this is of higher dimension than 2). Given a particular activity pattern 𝒑 (from a different epoch, in this example EA or SE) , 𝑷SA𝒑 is the projection of the pattern onto the subspace HSA. This is the component of the pattern which can be explained by the space HSA, and in the case where the pattern 𝒑 fully resides within HSA, 𝑷SA𝒑=𝒑. Conversely, (𝟙-𝑷SA)𝒑 is the component of the pattern which cannot be explained by the space HSA. It is the projection of the pattern 𝒑 onto the orthogonal complement of the subspace spanned by SA, HSA. (F) Projection of EA and SE patterns onto the orthogonal complement subspace spanned by SA patterns indicates that EA and SE patterns are less similar to SA over development. (G) Projection of EA and SA patterns onto the orthogonal complement subspace spanned by SE patterns indicates that EA and SA patterns are less similar to SE over development.
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. Matching recording lengths for evoked activity and spontaneous activity does not change the conclusions.
(A) For equal length of recording of TEA and SA (30 min), projection of EA and SE patterns onto the orthogonal complement or of the linear subspace spanned by SA patterns indicates that EA and SE patterns are less similar to SA over development. (B) For equal length of recording of TEA and SA (30 min), mean norm of the residual after projecting each EA and SA pattern onto the space spanned by SE patterns shows an increase in residual and indicates that EA and SA patterns are less similar to SE over development.

References

    1. Ackman JB, Crair MC. Role of emergent neural activity in visual map development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2014;24:166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.11.011. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ahrens MB, Li JM, Orger MB, Robson DN, Schier AF, Engert F, Portugues R. Brain-wide neuronal dynamics during motor adaptation in zebrafish. Nature. 2012;485:471–477. doi: 10.1038/nature11057. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Arroyo DA, Feller MB. Spatiotemporal features of retinal waves instruct the wiring of the visual circuitry. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2016;10:54. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00054. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Avitan L, Pujic Z, Hughes NJ, Scott EK, Goodhill GJ. Limitations of neural map topography for decoding spatial information. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:5385–5396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0385-16.2016. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Avitan L, Pujic Z, Mölter J, Van De Poll M, Sun B, Teng H, Amor R, Scott EK, Goodhill GJ. Spontaneous activity in the zebrafish tectum reorganizes over development and is influenced by visual experience. Current Biology. 2017;27:2407–2419. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.056. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources