Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2021 Apr 9:12:659434.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659434. eCollection 2021.

The Role of Language Proficiency in False Memory: A Mini Review

Affiliations
Review

The Role of Language Proficiency in False Memory: A Mini Review

Mar Suarez et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Memory errors and, specifically, false memories in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm have been extensively studied in the past decades. Most studies have investigated false memory in monolinguals' native or first language (L1), but interest has also grown in examining false memories in participants' second language (L2) with different proficiency levels. The main purpose of this manuscript is to review the current state of knowledge on the role of language proficiency on false memories when participants encode and retrieve information in the same language. To do so, a systematic literature search was conducted, and the available studies were reviewed. These studies differed in, for example, age, language proficiency, or material characteristics, including both high and low associative strength lists, and they reported different results. In this review, we attempted to make sense of the apparently contradictory results by carefully identifying participants' language dominance and L2 proficiency. Specifically, the results indicated that, first, people are more prone to produce false memories in their dominant than in their non-dominant language. This result generalizes to lists with high and low associative strength, as well as to participants of different ages. Second, false memories do not differ between two languages when speakers are equally proficient in both languages. Finally, highly proficient L2 speakers produce more false memories in their L2 than speakers with lower L2 proficiency. The results of this review will be considered in the light of the theoretical frameworks of false memories and bilingual language processing.

Keywords: DRM paradigm; bilingualism; false memories; false recognition; language proficiency.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abe N., Fujii T., Suzuki M., Ueno A., Shigemune Y., Mugikura S., et al. (2013). Encoding- and retrieval-related brain activity underlying false recognition. Neurosci. Res. 76 240–250. 10.1016/j.neures.2013.05.006 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aizpurua A., Garcia-Bajos E., Migueles M. (2009). False memories for a robbery in young and older adults. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 23 174–187. 10.1002/acp - DOI
    1. Albuquerque P. B. (2005). Produção de evocações e reconhecimentos falsos em 100 listas de palavras associadas portuguesas [False recall and recognition production in 100 Portuguese associate word list]. Lab. Psicol. 3 3–12. 10.14417/lp.766 - DOI
    1. Anastasi J. S., Rhodes M. G., Marquez S., Velino V. (2005). The incidence of false memories in native and non-native speakers. Memory 13 815–828. 10.1080/09658210444000421 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Anderson J. A. E., Mak L., Keyvani Chahi A., Bialystok E. (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behav. Res. Methods 50 250–263. 10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources