Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Observational Study
. 2021 Apr 13;13(4):1278.
doi: 10.3390/nu13041278.

Breakfast and Exercise Improve Academic and Cognitive Performance in Adolescents

Affiliations
Observational Study

Breakfast and Exercise Improve Academic and Cognitive Performance in Adolescents

Masato Kawabata et al. Nutrients. .

Abstract

This study examined the combined effects of breakfast and exercise on short-term academic and cognitive performance in adolescents. Eighty-two adolescents (64 female), aged 14-19 years, were randomized to four groups over a 4-hour morning: (i) a group who fasted and were sedentary (F-S); (ii) a group who ate breakfast but were sedentary (B-S); (iii) a group who fasted but completed a 30-min exercise bout (F-E); and (iv) a group who ate breakfast and completed a 30-min exercise bout (B-E). Individuals completed academic and cognitive tests over the morning. Adolescents in B-E significantly improved their mathematics score (B-E: 15.2% improvement on correct answers, vs. F-S: 6.7% improvement on correct answers; p = 0.014) and computation time for correct answers (B-E: 16.7% improvement, vs. F-S: 7.4% improvement; p = 0.004) over the morning compared with the F-S group. The B-E group had faster reaction times for congruent, incongruent and control trials of the Stroop Color-Word Task compared with F-S mid-morning (all p < 0.05). Morning breakfast and exercise combine to improve short-term mathematical task performance and speed in adolescents.

Keywords: breakfast; cognition; glycemic index; physical activity.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
A schematic representation of the study protocol. Exercise involved 8 min of treadmill running at a speed equivalent to 60% of peak oxygen uptake followed by 20 min of bilateral coordinative ball exercises. Breakfast contained 382 kcal and consisted of a sandwich and a chocolate malt powder drink. Three cognitive tests (Stroop Color-Word Test, Digit-Span Test, and Go/No-Go Task) were conducted in consecutive order for each test-series. The Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (3rd Edition) was used for academic tests of mathematical and oral word fluency for test-series1 and test-series3.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Blood glucose concentrations at the start (Pre-test1) and end of the morning (Post-test4) in the fasted and sedentary (F-S, ♦), breakfast and sedentary (B-S, ∆), fasted and exercise (F-E, □), and breakfast and exercise (B-E, ●) groups. 1 Change in B-E > than F-S over the morning, p < 0.05. 3 Change in B-E > than F-E over the morning, p < 0.05. Error bar: SD.
Figure 3
Figure 3
(a) Number of correct answers (mathematics score) and (b) correct answers per second (mathematics speed) in the fasted and sedentary (F-S, ♦), breakfast and sedentary (B-S, ∆), fasted and exercise (F-E, □), and breakfast and exercise (B-E, ●) groups in the 60 s arithmetic test during test-series1 and test-series3. 1 Change in B-E > than F-S over the test-series, p < 0.05. 3 Change in B-E > than F-E over the test-series, p < 0.05. 5 B-E different from F-E at test-series1, p < 0.05. Please note that there is no time lag in measurement among trials. Error bar: SD.
Figure 4
Figure 4
(a) Percentage change in mathematics score and (b) percentage change in mathematics speed in the fasted and sedentary (F-S), breakfast and sedentary (B-S), fasted and exercise (F-E), and breakfast and exercise (B-E) groups in the 60 s arithmetic test from test-series1 to test-series3. Error bars: 95% Confidence Intervals.

References

    1. Adolphus K., Lawton C.L., Champ C.L., Dye L. The Effects of Breakfast and Breakfast Composition on Cognition in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Adv. Nutr. 2016;7:590S–612S. doi: 10.3945/an.115.010256. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Benton D., Maconie A., Williams C. The influence of the glycaemic load of breakfast on the behaviour of children in school. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:717–724. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.065. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ingwersen J., Defeyter M.A., Kennedy D.O., Wesnes K.A., Scholey A.B. A low glycaemic index breakfast cereal preferentially prevents children’s cognitive performance from declining throughout the morning. Appetite. 2007;49:240–244. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.06.009. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mahoney C.R., Taylor H.A., Kanarek R.B., Samuel P. Effect of breakfast composition on cognitive processes in elementary school children. Physiol. Behav. 2005;85:635–645. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.023. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Wesnes K.A., Pincock C., Richardson D., Helm G., Hails S. Breakfast reduces declines in attention and memory over the morning in schoolchildren. Appetite. 2003;41:329–331. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2003.08.009. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types