Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Oct;31(10):1581-1590.
doi: 10.1007/s00787-021-01797-4. Epub 2021 May 13.

Fear conditioning and stimulus generalization in association with age in children and adolescents

Affiliations

Fear conditioning and stimulus generalization in association with age in children and adolescents

Julia Reinhard et al. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022 Oct.

Abstract

The aim of the study was to investigate age-related differences in fear learning and generalization in healthy children and adolescents (n = 133), aged 8-17 years, using an aversive discriminative fear conditioning and generalization paradigm adapted from Lau et al. (2008). In the current task, participants underwent 24 trials of discriminative conditioning of two female faces with neutral facial expressions, with (CS+) or without (CS-) a 95-dB loud female scream, presented simultaneously with a fearful facial expression (US). The discriminative conditioning was followed by 72 generalization trials (12 CS+, 12 GS1, 12 GS2, 12 GS3, 12 GS4, and 12 CS-): four generalization stimuli depicting gradual morphs from CS+ to CS- in 20%-steps were created for the generalization phases. We hypothesized that generalization in children and adolescents is negatively correlated with age. The subjective ratings of valence, arousal, and US expectancy (the probability of an aversive noise following each stimulus), as well as skin conductance responses (SCRs) were measured. Repeated-measures ANOVAs on ratings and SCR amplitudes were calculated with the within-subject factors stimulus type (CS+, CS-, GS1-4) and phase (Pre-Acquisition, Acquisition 1, Acquisition 2, Generalization 1, Generalization 2). To analyze the modulatory role of age, we additionally calculated ANCOVAs considering age as covariate. Results indicated that (1) subjective and physiological responses were generally lower with increasing age irrespective to the stimulus quality, and (2) stimulus discrimination improved with increasing age paralleled by reduced overgeneralization in older individuals. Longitudinal follow-up studies are required to analyze fear generalization with regard to brain maturational aspects and clarify whether overgeneralization of conditioned fear promotes the development of anxiety disorders or vice versa.

Keywords: Anxiety disorders; Childhood and adolescence; Development; Fear conditioning; Fear generalization; Overgeneralization.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Correlations between age and differential scores between CS+ and CS− in US expectancy ratings in (a) Acquisition 1 (ACQ1) and (b) Acquisition 2 (ACQ2). The significant positive correlation indicates better discrimination between CS+ and CS− in older participants after ACQ 1. There was no significant correlation with age, however, after ACQ 2
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Correlations between age and the generalization index (GI) score based on the US expectancy ratings. The negative correlation indicated that older participants showed reduced generalization of conditioned fear

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Costello EJ, Egger HL, Angold A. The developmental epidemiology of anxiety disorders: phenomenology, prevalence, and comorbidity. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2005;14(4):631–648. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2005.06.003. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Beesdo K, Knapp S, Pine DS. Anxiety and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: developmental issues and implications for DSM-V. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2009;32(3):483–524. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2009.06.002. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cartwright-Hatton S, Mc Nicol K, Doubleday E. Anxiety in neglected population: Prevalence of anxiety disorders in pre-adolescent children. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;27:817–833. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y. The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:56–64. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.56. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen H. Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the united states. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21(3):169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources