Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 May 11;11(5):1360.
doi: 10.3390/ani11051360.

Can Dogs' Origins and Interactions with Humans Affect Their Accomplishments? A Study on the Responses of Shelter and Companion Dogs during Vocal Cue Training

Affiliations

Can Dogs' Origins and Interactions with Humans Affect Their Accomplishments? A Study on the Responses of Shelter and Companion Dogs during Vocal Cue Training

Maria Luiza A Fonseca et al. Animals (Basel). .

Abstract

The inclusion of life history as a possible influential factor is pivotal in studies on behavior, welfare, and cognition. Shelter dogs have usually experienced a life involving poor social interactions with humans. Thus, we aimed to investigate the behavioral responses of shelter dogs (SDs) and companion dogs (CDs) during the training of two vocal cues ("sit", "paw"), as well as the possible associations between their responses and the behaviors of trainers. We studied 15 SDs and 15 CDs in up to eight five-minute training sessions. Dogs' and trainers' behaviors were recorded and analyzed (through GLM, GLMM, correlation and Mann-Whitney tests). Shelter dogs responded to more cues per session, with shorter latencies and fewer repetitions of cues. Moreover, SDs spent more time wagging their tails. Dogs' sex and trainers' behaviors were also associated with differences in dogs' responses. The use of a reproachful tone of voice was associated with a greater number of cues responded to, shorter latencies, and fewer repetitions of cues. However, this type voice/discourse was also linked to a greater exhibition of non-training behaviors (e.g., exploring the room or jumping on the trainer), and to dogs spending less time next to the trainer and wagging their tails. On the other hand, the use of a neutral tone of voice and laughter, besides being linked to performance, was also associated with longer durations of tail wagging. Furthermore, the duration of the trainers' orientation to dogs was correlated with the orientation of the dogs to the trainers. Our data suggest that, even when having experienced social deprivation from humans, SDs' capacities to learn vocal cues were preserved, possibly due to ontogenic homeostasis processes. Shelter dogs' greater interest in the sessions may be also credited to their socially-deprived routine. Our outcomes also point to an association between friendly interactions during training and dog performance and excitement, which suggests that such interactions may have the potential to improve SD welfare.

Keywords: dog cognition; dog–human interactions; life history; shelter dog; training; welfare.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Number of cues responded to (a), and cue repetition (“sit” and “paw”) (b) in the last training sessions of shelter dogs (SDs) and companion dogs (CDs). Bold horizontal lines represent medians; gray boxes represent quartiles; and thin horizontal lines depict minimum and maximum values.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Cue-repetitions in the last training sessions of shelter and companion dogs with Trainer 1 (T1) and Trainer 2 (T2). Bold horizontal lines show medians; gray boxes represent quartiles; thin horizontal lines show minimum and maximum values.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Duration of tail-wagging for shelter dogs (SDs) and companion dogs (CDs) during training sessions. Bold horizontal lines represent medians; gray boxes represent quartiles; thin horizontal lines represent minimum and maximum values.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Dispersion of the duration of trainers’ reproachful speech (a) and laughter (b) during training sessions with shelter and companion dogs as a function of the number of cues responded to per session.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Dispersion of the number of cues responded to by shelter and companion dogs per training session as a function of the duration of trainers’ gentle speech.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Howell T.J., Toukhsati S., Coiduit R., Bennett P. The Perceptions of Dog Intelligence and Cognitive Skills (PoDIaCS) Survey. J. Vet. Behav. 2013;8:418–424. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2013.05.005. - DOI
    1. Lea S.E.G., Osthaus B. In what sense are dogs special? Canine cognition in comparative context. Learn. Behav. 2018;46:335–363. doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0349-7. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Stewart L., MacLean E.L., Ivy D., Woods V., Cohen E., Rodriguez K., McIntyre M., Mukherjee S., Call J., Kaminski J., et al. Citizen Science as a New Tool in Dog Cognition Research. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0135176. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135176. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Kaminski J., Nitzschner M. Do dogs get the point? A review of dog-human communication ability. Learn. Motiv. 2013;44:294–302. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2013.05.001. - DOI
    1. Gácsi M., Györi B., Virányi Z., Kubinyi E., Range F., Belényi B., Miklósi Á. Explaining Dog Wolf Differences in Utilizing Human Pointing Gestures: Selection for Synergistic Shifts in the Development of Some Social Skills. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e0006584. doi: 10.1371/annotation/9d7a0174-3068-4c44-bb98-b8a9bc5a99d5. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources