Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 May 20:12:648586.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648586. eCollection 2021.

A Current View on Dual-Task Paradigms and Their Limitations to Capture Cognitive Load

Affiliations

A Current View on Dual-Task Paradigms and Their Limitations to Capture Cognitive Load

Shirin Esmaeili Bijarsari. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Dual-task paradigms encompass a broad range of approaches to measure cognitive load in instructional settings. As a common characteristic, an additional task is implemented alongside a learning task to capture the individual's unengaged cognitive capacities during the learning process. Measures to determine these capacities are, for instance, reaction times and interval errors on the additional task, while the performance on the learning task is to be maintained. Opposite to retrospectively applied subjective ratings, the continuous assessment within a dual-task paradigm allows to simultaneously monitor changes in the performance related to previously defined tasks. Following the Cognitive Load Theory, these changes in performance correspond to cognitive changes related to the establishment of permanently existing knowledge structures. Yet the current state of research indicates a clear lack of standardization of dual-task paradigms over study settings and task procedures. Typically, dual-task designs are adapted uniquely for each study, albeit with some similarities across different settings and task procedures. These similarities range from the type of modality to the frequency used for the additional task. This results in a lack of validity and comparability between studies due to arbitrarily chosen patterns of frequency without a sound scientific base, potentially confounding variables, or undecided adaptation potentials for future studies. In this paper, the lack of validity and comparability between dual-task settings will be presented, the current taxonomies compared and the future steps for a better standardization and implementation discussed.

Keywords: cognitive load; cognitive load measurement; comparability; dual task; measurement; secondary task; taxonomy; validity.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Holistic taxonomy of dual-task settings with exemplary selection paths.

References

    1. Antonenko P., Paas F., Grabner R., van Gog T. (2010). Using electroencephalography to measure cognitive load. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 22, 425–438. 10.1007/s10648-010-9130-y - DOI
    1. Ayres P., Cierniak G. (2012). “Split-attention effect” in Springer Reference. Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning. ed. Seel N. M. (Boston, MA: Springer US; ), 3172–3175.
    1. Brown I. D. (1978). Dual task methods of assessing work-load. Ergonomics 21, 221–224. 10.1080/00140137808931716 - DOI
    1. Brünken R., Plass J. L., Leutner D. (2003). Direct measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educ. Psychol. 38, 53–61. 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_7 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brünken R., Seufert T., Paas F. (2010). “Measuring cognitive load” in Cognitive Load Theory. eds. Plass J. L., Moreno R., Brünken R. (Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press; ), 181–202.