Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020;3(3):158-167.
doi: 10.1080/25742442.2021.1896908. Epub 2021 Mar 15.

Recall of Speech is Impaired by Subsequent Masking Noise: A Replication of Experiment 2

Affiliations

Recall of Speech is Impaired by Subsequent Masking Noise: A Replication of Experiment 2

Claire Guang et al. Audit Percept Cogn. 2020.

Abstract

Introduction: The presence of masking noise can impair speech intelligibility and increase the attentional and cognitive resources necessary to understand speech. The first study to demonstrate the negative cognitive effects of noisy speech found that participants had poorer recall for aurally-presented digits early in a list when later digits were presented in noise relative to quiet (Rabbitt, 1968). However, despite being cited nearly 500 times and providing the foundation for a wealth of subsequent research on the topic, the original study has never been directly replicated.

Methods: This study replicated Rabbitt (1968) with a large online sample and tested its robustness to a variety of analytical and scoring techniques.

Results: We replicated Rabbitt's key finding that listening to speech in noise impairs recall for items that came earlier in the list. The results were consistent when we used the original analytical technique (an ANOVA) and a more powerful analytical technique (generalized linear mixed effects models) that was not available when the original paper was published.

Discussion: These findings support the claim that effortful listening can interfere with encoding or rehearsal of previously presented information.

Keywords: listening effort; recall; replication; spoken word recognition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Figures

Figure 1:
Figure 1:
Left: Design schematic of the four noise conditions (clear/clear, noise/clear, clear/noise, noise/noise) in Rabbitt (1968). On each trial, participants heard the first half and the second half of each list in either noise or silence (“clear”) and were then cued to recall one or the other. Right: Example “clear-noise” trial in which participants heard the first half of the list without masking noise and the second half with noise and were then asked to recall the first half.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Violin plot depicting distribution of absolute recall of first-half lists (out of 7) by noise condition of second-half list. Black points indicate means for each condition, grey squares indicate the mean values reported in Rabbitt (1968).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Anwyl-Irvine AL, Massonnié J, Flitton A, Kirkham N, & Evershed JK (2020). Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 388–407. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, Christensen R, Singmann H, Dai B, Scheipl F, Grothendieck G, & Green P (2014). Package “lme4” (Version 1.1–15). R foundation for statistical computing, Vienna, 12. https://github.com/lme4/lme4/
    1. Bentley SV, Greenaway KH, & Haslam SA (2017). An online paradigm for exploring the self-reference effect. PloS One, 12(5), e0176611. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cousins KAQ, Dar H, Wingfield A, & Miller P (2014). Acoustic masking disrupts time-dependent mechanisms of memory encoding in word-list recall. Memory & Cognition, 42(4), 622–638. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Crump MJC, McDonnell JV, & Gureckis TM (2013). Evaluating Amazon’s Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PloS One, 8(3), e57410. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources