Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022;46(1):43-61.
doi: 10.1007/s10608-021-10256-y. Epub 2021 Jul 30.

No Evidence for the Involvement of Cognitive Immunisation in Updating Beliefs About the Self in Three Non-Clinical Samples

Affiliations

No Evidence for the Involvement of Cognitive Immunisation in Updating Beliefs About the Self in Three Non-Clinical Samples

Tobias Kube et al. Cognit Ther Res. 2022.

Abstract

Background: Cognitive immunisation against disconfirmatory evidence (i.e., devaluing expectation-disconfirming information through cognitive mechanisms) has recently been discussed as an obstacle to the revision of dysfunctional beliefs in mental disorders such as depression. Yet, it is unclear whether cognitive immunisation is also involved in belief updating in non-clinical samples.

Methods: Using a three-group modulation protocol (promotion vs. inhibition of cognitive immunisation vs. control group), we examined how cognitive immunisation influences belief updating in response to performance feedback in three non-clinical samples. In Experiments 1 (N = 99) and 2 (N = 93), participants received unexpectedly negative feedback, whereas participants from Experiment 3 (N = 118) received unexpectedly positive feedback. Depressive symptoms and dispositional optimism were examined as additional predictors of belief updating.

Results: In all experiments, participants adjusted their expectations in line with the feedback received, but this effect was not influenced by the cognitive immunisation manipulation. In Experiment 3, expectation change remained stable over 2 weeks. Depressive symptoms were associated with a reduced integration of positive feedback, but not with an increased sensitivity to negative feedback.

Conclusions: Whereas previous research has shown that cognitive immunisation contributes to persistent beliefs in clinical populations, the present findings suggest that it does not affect belief updating in non-clinical samples.

Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10608-021-10256-y.

Keywords: Belief updating; Cognitive immunisation; Depression; Expectation; Reappraisal.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of InterestTobias Kube and Julia Anna Glombiewski declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Design of “Experiment 1”
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Results for the main analyses from Experiment 1. There were no significant differences between the groups in updating their expectations from pre to post. Error bars reflect the standard error or the mean
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Results from Experiment 1 for expectation change when considering only the negatively worded expectation items (e.g., “Solving the tasks from the test will be difficult for me”). For generalised expectations, participants from the cognitive immunisation inhibition group updated their expectations in a negative direction, whereas participants from the cognitive immunisation promotion group did not. For task-specific expectations, it was found that participants from the cognitive immunisation promotion group updated their expectations to a lesser extent than participants from the cognitive immunisation inhibition group and the control group. Error bars reflect the standard error or the mean
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Results for the main analyses from Experiment 3. There were no significant differences between the groups in updating their expectations from pre to post to follow-up. Error bars reflect the standard error or the mean

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Barrett LF, Simmons WK. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16(7):419–430. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri WF. Comparison of beck depression inventories-IA and-II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1996;67(3):588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bless H, Schwarz N, Bodenhausen GV, Thiel L. Personalized versus generalized benefits of stereotype disconfirmation: Trade-offs in the evaluation of atypical exemplars and their social groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2001;37(5):386–397. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2000.1459. - DOI
    1. Blickle G, Momm T, Liu Y, Witzki A, Steinmayr R. Construct validation of the test of emotional intelligence (TEMINT): A two-study investigation. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2011;27(4):282. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000075. - DOI
    1. Brolsma SC, Vrijsen JN, Vassena E, Kandroodi MR, Bergman MA, van Eijndhoven PF, Collard RM, den Ouden HE, Schene AH, Cools R. Challenging the negative learning bias hypothesis of depression: reversal learning in a naturalistic psychiatric sample. Psychological Medicine. 2020 doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001956. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources