Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Aug 11;16(8):e0255346.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255346. eCollection 2021.

Trusting the experts: The domain-specificity of prestige-biased social learning

Affiliations

Trusting the experts: The domain-specificity of prestige-biased social learning

Charlotte O Brand et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Prestige-biased social learning (henceforth "prestige-bias") occurs when individuals predominantly choose to learn from a prestigious member of their group, i.e. someone who has gained attention, respect and admiration for their success in some domain. Prestige-bias is proposed as an adaptive social-learning strategy as it provides a short-cut to identifying successful group members, without having to assess each person's success individually. Previous work has documented prestige-bias and verified that it is used adaptively. However, the domain-specificity and generality of prestige-bias has not yet been explicitly addressed experimentally. By domain-specific prestige-bias we mean that individuals choose to learn from a prestigious model only within the domain of expertise in which the model acquired their prestige. By domain-general prestige-bias we mean that individuals choose to learn from prestigious models in general, regardless of the domain in which their prestige was earned. To distinguish between domain specific and domain general prestige we ran an online experiment (n = 397) in which participants could copy each other to score points on a general-knowledge quiz with varying topics (domains). Prestige in our task was an emergent property of participants' copying behaviour. We found participants overwhelmingly preferred domain-specific (same topic) prestige cues to domain-general (across topic) prestige cues. However, when only domain-general or cross-domain (different topic) cues were available, participants overwhelmingly favoured domain-general cues. Finally, when given the choice between cross-domain prestige cues and randomly generated Player IDs, participants favoured cross-domain prestige cues. These results suggest participants were sensitive to the source of prestige, and that they preferred domain-specific cues even though these cues were based on fewer samples (being calculated from one topic) than the domain-general cues (being calculated from all topics). We suggest that the extent to which people employ a domain-specific or domain-general prestige-bias may depend on their experience and understanding of the relationships between domains.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Three example screenshots representing what participants saw at different stages of the experiment.
The top screenshot is an example question from the language topic. Participants could either select one of the two blue buttons showing two possible answers (one correct, one incorrect), or select the red button labelled “Ask Someone Else” which allows participants to copy someone else within their group. The number ‘7’ at the bottom is a countdown timer that forces participants to answer within 15 seconds. The second image represents what a participant would see if they chose to “Ask Someone Else” in Round 2 of Condition C, where they could choose to either view Times Chosen Altogether (domain-general prestige) or Times Chosen On This Topic (domain-specific prestige). The bottom image represents what a participant would see if they chose ‘Times Chosen Altogether’ and (domain-general prestige), in which there were only two other players to choose from. Please note that for any given question, participants could have between one and nine other participants to choose from, depending on how many answered individually for that particular question. See Table 1 for the information combinations displayed in the other Conditions. See S1 File for screenshots for all Conditions.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Raw counts of the information chosen when participants chose to copy someone else’s answer in Round 2.
Total possible copying instances for each condition in Round 2 were: Condition A = 3240, Condition B = 3437, Condition C = 3377, Condition D = 3416.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Model predictions for participants choosing the predicted information compared to the alternative information in Round 2 of the four conditions, on the probability scale.

References

    1. Henrich J, McElreath R. The evolution of cultural evolution. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev. 2003;12: 123–135. doi: 10.1002/evan.10110 - DOI
    1. Kendal RL, Boogert NJ, Rendell L, Laland KN, Webster M, Jones PL. Social Learning Strategies: Bridge-Building between Fields. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22: 651–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Rendell L, Fogarty L, Hoppitt WJE, Morgan TJH, Webster MM, Laland KN. Cognitive culture: theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15: 68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.002 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mesoudi A. An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evol Hum Behav. 2011;32: 334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.12.001 - DOI
    1. Morgan TJH, Rendell LE, Ehn M, Hoppitt W, Laland KN. The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proc R Soc B. 2012;279: 653–662. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1172 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources