Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Nov;239(11):3221-3242.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06191-9. Epub 2021 Aug 27.

Posterior brain sensorimotor recruitment for inhibition of delayed responses in children

Affiliations

Posterior brain sensorimotor recruitment for inhibition of delayed responses in children

Kristina T R Ciesielski et al. Exp Brain Res. 2021 Nov.

Abstract

Inhibitory control, the ability to suppress irrelevant thoughts or actions, is central to cognitive and social development. Protracted maturation of frontal brain networks has been reported as a major restraint for this ability, yet, young children, when motivated, successfully inhibit delayed responses. A better understanding of the age-dependent neural inhibitory mechanism operating during the awaiting-to-respond window in children may elucidate this conundrum. We recorded ERPs from children and parental adults to a visual-spatial working memory task with delayed responses. Cortical activation elicited during the first 1000 ms of the awaiting-to-respond window showed, as predicted by prior studies, early inhibitory effects in prefrontal ERPs (P200, 160-260 ms) associated with top-down attentional-biasing, and later effects in parietal/occipital ERPs (P300, 270-650 ms) associated with selective inhibition of task-irrelevant stimuli/responses and recurrent memory retrieval. Children successfully inhibited delayed responses and performed with a high level of accuracy (often over 90%), although, the prefrontal P200 displayed reduced amplitude and uniformly delayed peak latency, suggesting low efficacy of top-down attentional-biasing. P300, however, with no significant age-contrasts in latency was markedly elevated in children over the occipital/inferior parietal regions, with effects stronger in younger children. These results provide developmental evidence supporting the sensorimotor recruitment model of visual-spatial working memory relying on the occipital/parietal regions of the early maturing dorsal-visual network. The evidence is in line with the concept of age-dependent variability in the recruitment of cognitive inhibitory networks, complementing the former predominant focus on frontal lobes.

Keywords: Dorsal visual network; Inhibition of delayed responses; Occipital/parietal P300; Posterior brain sensorimotor recruitment; Prefrontal P200; Typical development.

PubMed Disclaimer

Similar articles

References

    1. Adleman NE, Menon V, Blasey CM, White CD, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, Reiss AL (2002) A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color-word task. Neuroimage 16:61–75 - PubMed
    1. Alexander J, Bauer L, Kuperman S, Morzorati S, O’Connor S, Rohbraugh J, Porjesz B, Begleiter H, Polich J (1996) Hemispheric differences for P300 amplitude from an auditory oddball task. Int J Psychophysiol 21:189–196 - PubMed
    1. Anastasiades PG, Marques-Smith A, Lyngholm D, Lickiss T, Raffiq S, Kätzel D, Butt SJ (2016) GABAergic interneurons form transient layer-specific circuits in early postnatal neocortex. Nat Commun 7:10584 - PubMed - PMC
    1. Anllo-Vento L, Luck SJ, Hillyard SA (1998) Spatio-temporal dynamics of attention to color: evidence from human electrophysiology. Hum Brain Mapp 6(4):216–238 - PubMed - PMC
    1. Apicella P, Scarnati E, Ljungberg T, Schultz W (1992) Neuronal activity in monkey striatum related to the expectation of predictable environmental events. J Neurophysiol 68(3):945–960 - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources