Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Mar/Apr;43(2):531-544.
doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000001116.

Acoustic Change Complex and Visually Reinforced Infant Speech Discrimination Measures of Vowel Contrast Detection

Affiliations

Acoustic Change Complex and Visually Reinforced Infant Speech Discrimination Measures of Vowel Contrast Detection

Barbara K Cone et al. Ear Hear. 2022 Mar/Apr.

Abstract

Objectives: To measure the effect of stimulus rate and vowel change direction on the acoustic change complex (ACC) latencies and amplitudes and compare ACC metrics to behavioral measures of vowel contrast detection for infants tested under the age of 1 year. We tested the hypothesis that the direction of spectral energy shift from a vowel change would result in differences in the ACC, owing to the sensitivity of cortical neurons to the direction of frequency change. We evaluated the effect of the stimulus rate (1/s versus 2/s) on the infants' ACC. We evaluated the ACC amplitude ratio's sensitivity (proportion of ACCs present for each change trial) and compared it to perceptual responses obtained using a visually reinforced infant speech discrimination paradigm (VRISD). This report provides normative data from infants for the ACC toward the ultimate goal of developing a clinically useful index of neural capacity for vowel discrimination.

Design: Twenty-nine infants, nine females, 4.0 to 11.8 months of age, participated. All participants were born at full term and passed their newborn hearing screens. None had risk factors for hearing or neurologic impairment. Cortical auditory evoked potentials were obtained in response to synthesized vowel tokens /a/, /i/, /o/, and /u/ presented at a rate of 1- or 2/s in an oddball stimulus paradigm with a 25% probability of the deviant stimulus. All combinations of vowel tokens were tested at the two rates. The ACC was obtained in response to the deviant stimulus. The infants were also tested for vowel contrast detection using a VRISD paradigm with the same combinations of vowel tokens used for the ACC. The mean age at the time of the ACC test was 5.4 months, while the mean age at the behavioral test was 6.8 months.

Results: Variations in ACC amplitude and latency occurred as a function of the initial vowel token and the contrast token. However, the hypothesis that the direction of vowel (spectral) change would result in significantly larger change responses for high-to-low spectral changes was not supported. The contrasts with /a/ as the leading vowel of the contrast pair resulted in the largest ACC amplitudes than other conditions. Significant differences in the ACC presence and amplitude were observed as a function of rate, with 2/s resulting in ACCs with the largest amplitude ratios. Latency effects of vowel contrast and rate were present, but not systematic. The ACC amplitude ratio's sensitivity for detecting a vowel contrast was greater for the 2/s rate than the 1/s rate. For an amplitude ratio criterion of ≥1.5, the sensitivity was 93% for ACC component P2-N2 at 2/s, whereas at 1/s sensitivity was 70%. VRISD tests of vowel-contrast detection had a 71% hit and a 21% false-positive rate. Many infants who could not reach performance criteria for VRISD had ACC amplitude ratios of ≥2.0.

Conclusions: The ACC for vowel contrasts presented at a rate of 2/s is a robust index of vowel-contrast detection when obtained in typically developing infants under the age of 1 year. The ACC is present in over 90% of infants tested at this rate when an amplitude ratio criterion of ≥1.5 is used to define a response. The amplitude ratio appears to be a sensitive metric for the difference between a control and contrast condition. The ACC can be obtained in infants who do not yet exhibit valid behavioral responses for vowel change contrasts and may be useful for estimating neural capacity for discriminating these sounds.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Figures

Figure 1:
Figure 1:
Vowel Spectra: Spectral energy by third octave band (20–8000 Hz) for synthesized vowel stimuli. (Cheek and Cone, 2020, with permission).
Figure 2:
Figure 2:
Vowel token envelope characteristics and stimulus paradigm. The vowel-token envelope had 10 ms linear rise-fall times and 480 ms plateau duration for a total duration of 500 ms. The oddball stimulus paradigm for the control and contrast test conditions for the /i/-/u/) contrasts is illustrated. The MMN/P300 module allows for separate averaging of responses to standard tokens presented with 75% probability into one buffer (A) and responses to the deviant tokens presented at 25% probability into another buffer (B). In the control condition, the “deviant” token is the same as the standard (/i/), but sampled and averaged over 25% of the stimulus presentations into memory buffer B. In the contrast condition, the deviant vowel token, /u/, is different from the standard (/i) occurs with a 25% probability and the response is averaged into memory buffer B.
Figure 3:
Figure 3:
Examples of CAEP onset and ACC responses from individual infants for contrasts /i/, /o/ and /u/. Waveform key: formula image CAEP onset response to standard vowel token formula image ACC response in control condition formula image ACC response in contrast conditions, /a/, /o/ /i/ or /u/ ACC waveforms shown in alphabetical order for each condition: for /i/, the contrasts shown are /a/, /o/ and /u/. For /o/, the contrast are /a/, /i/ and /u/. For /u/, the contrasts are /a/, /i/, /o/. Markers have been provided for response components in some waveforms as a guide for identifying the components in the subsequent waveforms. ACCs are identified as differences in amplitude between the control (thin gray trace) and contrast (thin black trace) conditions. In the 1/s example, ACCs for the /u/ condition are questionable for the /i/ and /o/ (bottom two) traces. As described in the text, CAEP onset responses to the standard vowel token are frequetly absent at the 2/s rate and this is the case in the examples found for /o/ and /u/. Examples of waveforms for /a/ standards, controls and /i/, /o/ and /u/ contrasts are found in Cone (2015).
Figure 4:
Figure 4:
Percent detected components for P1-N1, N1-P2 and P2-N2 for 1) onset responses to the standard token; and 2) ACC responses in control and contrast conditions. ANY refers the percent of trials in which any CAEP or ACC component was present, and all absent refers to the percent of trials in which no identifiable components were present. The embedded table indicates the number of test trials for each vowel pair, at each rate.
Figure 5:
Figure 5:
Mean peak-to-trough amplitudes of ACC responses in control and contrast conditions, A= 1/s rate and B = 2/s rate. Error bars indicate ± 1 standard error.
Figure 6:
Figure 6:
Mean ACC amplitude ratios as a function of rate. When means are calculated with values >0 μV for the contrast condition, the mean amplitude ratios increase for each rate. Mean amplitude ratios at 2/s are greater than amplitude ratios at 1/s for all calculations. Error bars indicate ± 1 standard error.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Casey KA, Small SA (2014) Comparisons of auditory steady-state response and behavioral air conduction and bone conduction thresholds for infants and adults with normal hearing. Ear and Hearing 35(4): 423–439. - PubMed
    1. Cheek D, Cone B (2020) Evidence of vowel discrimination provided by the acoustic change complex. Ear and Hearing 41(4) 855–867. - PubMed
    1. Ching TY, Zhang VW, Hou S, Van Buynder P (2016) Cortical evoked potentials reveal changes in audibility with nonlinear frequency compression in hearing aids for children: clinical implications. Seminars in Hearing. 37(1):25–35, - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cheour M, Lepannen PH, Kraus N (2000) Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children. Clinical Neurophysiology 111 (1), 4–16. - PubMed
    1. Cone BK (2015) Infant cortical electrophysiology and perception of vowel contrasts. International Journal of Psychophysiology 95: 65–76. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types