Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2021 Sep 27:12:747854.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.747854. eCollection 2021.

Affect Dysregulation in Context: Implications and Future Directions of Experience Sampling Research on Affect Regulation Models of Loss of Control Eating

Affiliations
Review

Affect Dysregulation in Context: Implications and Future Directions of Experience Sampling Research on Affect Regulation Models of Loss of Control Eating

Megan E Mikhail. Front Psychiatry. .

Abstract

Loss of control eating is a core, transdiagnostic eating disorder symptom associated with psychological distress, functional impairment, and reduced quality of life. However, the factors that contribute to persistent loss of control eating despite negative consequences are not fully understood. Understanding the mechanisms that maintain loss of control eating is crucial to advance treatments that interrupt these processes. Affect regulation models of loss of control eating hypothesize that negative emotions trigger loss of control eating, and that loss of control eating is negatively reinforced because it temporarily decreases negative affect. Several variations on this basic affect regulation model have been proposed, including theories suggesting that negative affect decreases during loss of control eating rather than afterwards (escape theory), and that loss of control eating replaces one negative emotion with another that is less aversive (trade-off theory). Experience sampling designs that measure negative affect and eating behavior multiple times per day are optimally suited to examining the nuanced predictions of these affect regulation models in people's everyday lives. This paper critically reviews experience sampling studies examining associations between negative affect and loss of control eating, and discusses the implications for different affect regulation models of loss of control eating. The review concludes by proposing an expanded affect-focused model of loss of control eating that incorporates trait-level individual differences and momentary biological and environmental variables to guide future research. Clinical implications and recommendations are discussed.

Keywords: binge eating; ecological momentary assessment; emotion regulation; experience sampling; loss of control eating; negative affect.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Current affect regulation models of negative affect (NA) and loss of control eating (LOC). In the standard affect regulation model, NA increases prior to LOC, and decreases following LOC. Conversely, escape theory predicts that NA decreases while LOC is in progress, but increases to pre-LOC levels after LOC. Restraint theory predicts that NA disrupts the cognitive control required to maintain dietary restraint, leading to LOC. Trade-off theory predicts that LOC replaces one negative emotion (e.g., anger) with another that is less aversive (e.g., sadness). Finally, expectancy theory predicts that people engage in LOC because they expect to feel better afterwards, even though they may not actually feel better.
Figure 2
Figure 2
A revised affect-focused model of loss of control eating (LOC). The central figure shows the nomothetic pattern of changes in negative affect (NA) surrounding LOC found in a majority of experience sampling studies. This model is nestled within several layers of biological and environmental factors that could potentially alter the strength of NA-LOC associations. First are momentary biological factors, such as changes in hormone levels and blood sugar, that may alter reactivity to unpleasant events or stimuli. Second are trait-level individual differences that may moderate NA-LOC associations. Third are proximal environmental factors, such as interactions with peers or family members, that may increase (or decrease) NA, or affect the likelihood that NA will lead to LOC. Finally, individuals are encompassed by a broader sociocultural context.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Brownstone LM, Bardone-Cone AM. Subjective binge eating: a marker of disordered eating and broader psychological distress. Eat Weight Disord. (2020) 1–9. 10.1007/s40519-020-01053-9 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Kessler RC, Berglund PA, Chiu WT, Deitz AC, Hudson JI, Shahly V, et al. . The prevalence and correlates of binge eating disorder in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 73:904–14. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.020 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Rieger E, Wilfley DE, Stein RI, Marino V, Crow SJ. A comparison of quality of life in obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. (2005) 37:234–40. 10.1002/eat.20101 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behav Res Ther. (2003) 41:509–28. 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mitchison D, Touyz S, González-Chica DA, Stocks N, Hay P. How abnormal is binge eating? 18-year time trends in population prevalence and burden. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2017) 136:147–55. 10.1111/acps.12735 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources