Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023;32(1):123-168.
doi: 10.1007/s11191-021-00288-x. Epub 2021 Nov 16.

Long-Lasting Conceptual Change in Science Education: The Role of U-shaped Pattern of Argumentative Dialogue in Collaborative Argumentation

Affiliations

Long-Lasting Conceptual Change in Science Education: The Role of U-shaped Pattern of Argumentative Dialogue in Collaborative Argumentation

Xiaoshan Li et al. Sci Educ (Dordr). 2023.

Abstract

Meaningful learning for conceptual change in science education should aim to help students change their existing misconceptions to develop an accurate understanding of scientific concepts. Although collaborative argumentation is assumed to support such processes, its value for conceptual change is unclear. Moreover, the roles of argumentative dialogue should be considered in studies on collaborative argumentation. In the present study, using a controlled experiment, we examined the value of collaborative argumentation for conceptual change in science education while fully considering the roles of argumentative dialogue. Twenty-three postgraduate students were each allocated to one of two conditions (individual argumentation [control group] and collaborative argumentation [experimental group]) and participated in two argumentation activities. The results revealed that collaborative argumentation had a delayed but long-lasting effect on conceptual change in science education (i.e., conceptual change induced by collaborative argumentation did not immediately indicate a significant improvement at the moments of argumentation but showed a significant improvement during the delay period). Collaborative argumentation provided opportunities for change in cognitive, ontological, intentional, and other aspects of learning. Dialogue protocol analysis revealed that long-lasting conceptual change was associated with a U-shaped pattern of argumentative dialogue (i.e., two high and one low: both deliberative argumentation and co-consensual construction frequently occurred, while disputative argumentation rarely occurred) in collaborative argumentation. A third argumentation activity was then conducted to confirm this unexpected finding. The results confirmed an association between long-lasting conceptual change and a U-shaped pattern of argumentative dialogue in collaborative argumentation. The current study sheds light on the value of collaborative argumentation for long-lasting conceptual change, deepening our understanding of whether conceptual gains from argumentation activities were contingent on a particular type of verbal dialogue powered by collaborative argumentation. Implications for science education were discussed.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of InterestThe authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Two versions of worksheets. (a) Collaborative argumentation version of worksheet; (b) individual argumentation version of worksheet. Full lines indicate the difference between the two versions of worksheets, and dotted lines indicate the similarity
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
The coloring task for the first argumentation activity in the Star of Questionnaires platform. (a) Instructions for the coloring item: please use your pencil to depict the gradual change in wing coloring in the five periods of the Industrial Revolution. (b) The original version of coloring item. (c) Students drew five simple circles to represent the peppered moths and used a pencil to shade the circles to represent the magnitude of change in peppered moth coloring. (d) When students completed the coloring task, they took photos of their work and uploaded the photos to the Star of Questionnaires platform
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Overview of research design
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Procedure of the three argumentation activities
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Example of a two-tier item in a conceptual understanding test. (a) A short description of a species; (b) five true/false statements that targeted five scientific concepts from theories related to the given topic. Students were instructed to judge each statement and further explain why it was correct or incorrect
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Example of an open-ended construction item on a conceptual understanding test. (a) A short description of a natural phenomenon that occurred in a species; (b) students were instructed to give a full explanation of how the given natural phenomenon was generated and developed
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
Comparison of conceptual understanding between students in the collaborative argumentation condition and students in the individual argumentation condition for the first and second argumentation activities. (a) The first argumentation activity. (b) The second argumentation activity. *indicates p < .05, **indicates p < .01, ***indicates p < .001
Fig. 8
Fig. 8
Comparison of gain scores (conceptual change) between students in the collaborative argumentation condition and students in the individual argumentation condition for the first and second argumentation activities. (a) The first argumentation activity; (b) the second argumentation activity. *indicates p < .05, **indicates p < .01, ***indicates p < .001
Fig. 9
Fig. 9
Comparing argumentative dialogue between the high- and low-performing groups for the first and second argumentation activities. (a) For the first argumentation activity, the high- and low-performing groups differed in argumentative dialogue (χ2 [1, 2] = 6.973, p < .05), and the high-performing group exhibited a U-shaped pattern of argumentative dialogue; (b) for the second argumentative activity, high- and low-performing groups differed in argumentative dialogue (χ2 [1, 2] = 6.202, p < .05), and the high-performing group exhibited a U-shaped pattern of argumentative dialogue. *indicates p < .05, **indicates p < .01, ***indicates p < .001
Fig. 10
Fig. 10
Comparing argumentative dialogue between the high- and low-performing groups for the third argumentation activities. High- and low-performing groups differed in argumentative dialogue χ2 (1, 2) = 14.962, p = .001, and the high-performing group exhibited a U-shaped pattern of argumentative dialogue. *indicates p < .05, **indicates p < .01, ***indicates p < .001

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Anderson CW, Smith EL. Richardson-Koehler, Educators’ Hand- book: A research perspective. Longman, Inc.; 1987. Teaching science; pp. 84–111.
    1. Archila PA, Molina J, Truscott de Mejía AM. Using historical scientific controversies to promote undergraduates’ argumentation. Science & Education. 2020;29:647–671. doi: 10.1007/s11191-020-00126-6. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Asterhan CS. Epistemic and interpersonal dimensions of peer argumentation. Affective learning together; 2013. pp. 251–271.
    1. Asterhan CS, Babichenko M. The social dimension of learning through argumentation: Effects of human presence and discourse style. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2015;107(3):740. doi: 10.1037/edu0000014. - DOI
    1. Asterhan CS, Dotan A. Feedback that corrects and contrasts students’ erroneous solutions with expert ones improves expository instruction for conceptual change. Instructional Science. 2018;46(3):337–355. doi: 10.1007/s11251-017-9441-1. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources